«not guilty» advertising slogan

Preaching, Accusation, Guilt and Denial: Learning from Ignaz Semmelweis for Climate Communication

Climate preaching, the proposal that consumers should voluntarily and individually protect the climate, induces a feeling of guilt that elicits climate denial and the observed reluctance to communicate the problem —climate silence. It is argued that climate preaching is therefore overall detrimental. The implicit induction of guilt and its rejection is a major reason for difficulties to communicate on climate change. The argument is exemplified by the story of Ignaz Semmelweis, the pioneer of evidence-based medicine who was born exactly two hundred years ago.

Preface

There are two reasons for writing or reading this article: Its arguments are important —if they are acknowledged. So far, nobody has written about them with consequence, as far as I can see.

The observations described and the thoughts presented in this text were collected and have matured over decades. However, the contemplation of existing results from scientific work and the writing of the text took less than one year.

The text was made available online in an early stage, then expanded considerably, resulting in a medley of arguments and much repetition. To improve on this, it was structured in four parts.

In Part One I summarize the tragic case of science denial of the 19th century. It caused the death (in agony) of millions of childbearing women. The title reflects the relative importance Semmelweis‘ story had in early versions of the article. If the title were chosen anew, it would rather be ‹the guilt complex on climate change› or similar.

In Part Two I point out the similarities between the rejection of Semmelweis‘ findings and the denial of climate change. There are several good books and many articles that explain the primary cause of climate denial, the deliberate deception led by the fossil fuel industry.

This article addresses the question why it was so easy for the deception campaign to spread doubt and disbelief. Why, for instance, do so many join in to spread doubt without being financed by big coal, big oil and gas? The induction of guilt, the avoidance of the unpleasant feeling and the psychological rejection of implicit accusations are key to understanding our difficulties to communicate on climate change.

In Part Three it is argued that appeals to consumers to voluntarily reduce their carbon footprint are guilt-inducing implicit accusations and therefore contribute to denial as well as to our reticence to talk about global warming. These two negative consequences of the appeals alone should be reason enough to refrain from appealing for individual consumer action.

The proposal is new, perhaps original, at least in this degree of thoroughness. Not everybody will find the proposal to refrain from climate appeals immediately compelling. Ultimately, the inevitable question is this: Should climate preaching (as I have decided to call the proposal of voluntary individual consumer action) be done and encouraged or should it be abstained from, discouraged and rejected? As so often, the answer to this question could be: it depends.

It depends on the audience addressed, one could claim with good reason. True, not everybody is affected by the appeals in the same way. However, the appeals reach everybody. In the end, we cannot really select our audience. To preach or not to preach on climate change, that is therefore the ultimate question this article addresses.

There may be more reasons to reject or welcome appeals for voluntary individual acts than the preaching’s negative effect on denial and climate silence. This is also discussed in the rather consequential third part of the article. There may, however, also be positive impacts of climate preaching.

In Part Four it is argued that the net effect of climate preaching is negative because possible positive effects are too small to compensate for the appeals‘ negative effects. Even, the supposedly positive effects may in fact rather be negative effects.

Perhaps, I should point out that I excluded one variant or outcome in my considerations. (Or, maybe I should not point it out because the outcome is extremely remote.) Whether the net effect of climate preaching will ultimately be negative or positive depends on how humanity will cope with carbon emissions —or continue to fail to contain them.

If there is continued failure to put effective policy in place to stop CO2 emissions but, instead of policy, a further development towards declaring and considering personal consumption resulting in carbon emissions a moral sin; if, consequently, a corresponding new moral belief becomes universally accepted and complied with, like a strictly followed religion, then climate preaching would be positive. It is a nightmarish scenario. To solve the climate crisis would take a long time —if it wouldn’t virtually take forever.

Consequently, the effects of climate change would be devastating. I am not sure who would have to be blamed for it, the climate preachers or those who fail to act politically on climate change by being a responsible politician or by putting pressure on politicians. Yet, the climate preachers, their followers and those who don’t act politically are often the same people.

To treat climate change as a personal, moral challenge, not a societal, political challenge, is a welcome subterfuge to escape our real responsibility. It is an excuse to dodge the need and effort to address the problem of climate change effectively, in due time. The psychology described and decried in this article serves to justify these excuses.

My arguments may psychologically be difficult to absorb. Their importance should nevertheless be recognized. The labeling of the text as «opinion» and the inclusion of some personal anecdotes are not meant to undermine the weight of the presented claims.

As particular as my claims and concerns may appear, I am not all alone with them. Albeit scarce, some existing research as well as books and articles by climate communication specialists already strongly support my initial claims (made in the second part). Also, friends and specialists commented positively on drafts and early editions of this article, a piece that has become the size of a small book. However, some reactions (a peculiar reaction) also make clear that my arguments are not only welcome.

There is a hesitation to spread and accept the arguments presented in this article because the message won’t likely be liked. Paradoxically, the acceptance and deployment of the arguments are hindered by essentially the same problem the article itself describes. The text may be perceived as an accusation and its arguments are therefore not likely to propagate with ease.

To accept the importance of the «guilt complex on climate change» one must be ready to view climate preaching with a critical eye and even view voluntary individual consumer action critically —at least to some extent. For many, this is very difficult, psychologically.

As in the case of Ignaz Semmelweis and his colleague doctors, those who should embrace my arguments most eagerly, or at least should take them into unbiased consideration, are also the most likely to feel offended. It can be anticipated that many will find the arguments personally inconvenient, qualify them as unjustified assertions, consciously or subconsciously perceive them as accusations and reject the arguments before giving them due consideration.

However, be assured, this piece is not meant to be an accusation. It is meant to help solve a problem. I can only hope that readers, specifically climate communicators, will read with an open mind. I wish they consider both the hypotheses and the supporting research and will find them helpful for their work and our common cause. Nearly everybody should be a climate communicator, but only few are.

Introduction

A personal account of denial

It was around 1978. I was busy spray-painting my kayak, when someone pointed out to me I was also busy destroying the ozone layer. It could be read in the newspapers, she said. I did not immediately believe her. For a moment I had fallen victim to what is defined below as the accusation rejection bias.

They won!

The government of the US, the world’s most powerful nation, both militarily and economically, has been taken over by climate (science) deniers. As if this alone were not terrible enough: They were elected democratically. This success by science deniers is unprecedented.

The takeover happened more than 150 years after the radiation property of carbon dioxide, which is responsible for its greenhouse effect, was first measured in a laboratory and more than one hundred years after the effect of augmented atmospheric CO2 concentrations was first quantified and predicted on the geophysical level —by mere brainpower, paper and pen.

Caveats also occurred: Oceans will take up CO2. Absorption bands of water vapor and CO2 overlap. Clouds fully moderate greenhouse gas induced warming; aerosols do; etc. These caveats indicate a psychological pattern, a desire to discover a bright side of the situation and communicate it. The caveats were nevertheless all dispelled.

Thirty years ago, James Hansen made precise predictions and testified before the same congress that is now dominated by outright deniers.

We now experience the unequivocal confirmation of old, essentially undisputed science. The global average temperature ventures into a realm never experienced by human civilizations. Unless effective political action is taken, it will, in the foreseeable future, increase to degrees never ever experienced by any creature of the genus homo.

We are witnessing the first devastating irreversible effects of greenhouse gas induced warming and CO2 in the oceans: the destruction of coral reefs, the ecosystems on which much if not most of the diversity of life in the oceans depends. Unless effective political action is taken, tropical corals will, in the rather near future, be gone.

The deniers won. Maybe they won even more than their originators wanted them to win. The sides are divided and continue to get polarized.

At least for now, the deniers go stronger than ever and the other side is largely paralyzed. While all this happened and happens, there is a strange silence. Politicians, neighbors and even scientists are reluctant to talk about climate change.

Why is that? What has gone wrong?

It is overdue for climate communicators to thoroughly analyze our difficulties, our approaches to communicate the problem, to rethink our acts —and readjust.


→ Jump to part 2 – Impact of Guilt Induction and Guilt Avoidance on Climate Communication


Part 1 – Semmelweis‘ Challenge

Remembering Ignaz Semmelweis

August 13 should be highlighted in every climate communicator’s agenda. With his name, Ignaz Semmelweis epitomizes the first important case of denial of modern science. Like the denial of climate change, it had devastating consequences. It cost the lives of millions of mostly young women who died in agony.

The denial was due to the same psychological factors that now cause the denial of climate change.

Semmelweis was a victim, too. Disregarded and vilified he, too, died in agony; on August 13 in 1865, in the Landesirrenanstalt Döbling, an asylum for the mentally deranged near Vienna.

Ferdinand Hebra had lured Semmelweis to Vienna to commit him to the asylum where Semmelweis was put in a straitjacket and a darkened cell. Hebra was a leading capacity in his field, dermatology, and one of Semmelweis‘ teachers in medicine. As the editor of the Viennese medical journal, Hebra had announced Semmelweis‘ breakthrough discovery in obstetrics (the branch of medicine that treats childbirth) and had given it due acclaim. The document which, seventeen years later, attested Semmelweis a mental disease had been signed by János Balassa, Semmelweis‘ house doctor, an internationally recognized authority in plastic surgery and a pioneer of cardiac resuscitation.

Ignaz Semmelweis in 1860
Ignaz Semmelweis in 1860 | Copper engraving by Jenö Doby.

An autopsy of Semmelweis‘ body was carried out by Carl Braun, who succeeded Semmelweis at the maternity clinic of Vienna’s Allgemeines Krankenhaus only a few years after Semmelweis‘ landmark discovery. Braun was Semmelweis‘ nemesis, both in Vienna and later in Budapest. By then, Semmelweis had few friends among obstetricians. (Braun and Hebra were awarded the honor of knighthood in 1877, at a time when germ theory had proven Semmelweis right.)

Multiple bone fractures were inflicted upon Semmelweis, supposedly by his guards, when he was forcefully delivered to the asylum. The fractures were neither reported by Braun nor by anyone else of his time. They were revealed a century later, in 1963, following an exhumation of Semmelweis‘ bodily remains. The mistreatment was not responsible for Semmelweis‘ death, however.

Ironically —or perhaps perfidiously— he was killed by an infectious disease, similar or even essentially equal to puerperal sepsis, also known as childbed fever. It remains unknown whether the infection was accidental or deliberately inflicted. The prevalence of inconsistencies around his death supports the hypothesis that Semmelweis was murdered.

The degree, nature and cause of his mental illness also remains unclear. An advanced state of syphilis or Alzheimer’s disease are being hypothesized among other candidate illnesses. It also remains unclear to what extent ignorance, indifference and rejection had made Semmelweis lose his mind.

Vienna’s Allgemeines Krankenhaus was among the leading hospitals for medical treatment and research. In an attempt to contain infanticide —or perhaps rather prostitution— women were urged to give birth in hospitals.

Admission to the obstetrics clinic was free of charge for the pregnant. Nevertheless, pretending to not have been making it to the hospital in time, many preferred to bear their children in the street. Their chances of survival were much higher if they stayed clear of hospitals, where often one in ten mothers died in their childbed. The death rate could be twice as high, during months.

From at least ancient Greece onwards, until Semmelweis‘ time, medical wisdom was dominated by the belief that an alleged equilibrium of four bodily fluids was key to the health (and temperament) of a patient. Failures of the theory were systematically excused by the pretense that every medical case was as individual as was the patient.

The advances by Louis Pasteur and others still lay a couple of decades in the future. However, in Semmelweis‘ time as a doctor in Vienna, medical practices and knowledge had already progressed beyond mere superstition and false excuses for shortcomings.

  • The existence of transmitting diseases was well accepted.
  • Vaccinations with cowpox against smallpox, invented in China in the 16th century, were applied widely.
  • Decades before Ignaz Semmelweis‘ discovery, the Italian Agostino Bassi had proven with experiments that a microscopic «vegetable parasite» —a fungus, really— caused a disease in silkworms which in turn devastated the French silk industry.
  • Semmelweis wrote about the similarity —and difference— of contracting diseases transmitted directly from one person to another and the indirect transmission he discovered. Albeit not yet declared, and far from explained, germ theory was in the making.

Three years before Semmelweis‘ discovery, US American physician and writer Oliver Wendell Holmes had already strongly proposed that puerperal fever could be transmitted via doctors, their hands and instruments and that hygiene was key to the prevention of the disease. He did not focus on hand hygiene as much as Semmelweis. But he was also confronted with the dismissal and rejection of his findings, essentially for the same reason.

Charles Meigs, another US obstetrician, who objected Holmes stated that doctors were gentlemen and gentlemen’s hands were clean. That was in 1854, 10 years after Holmes‘ and seven years after the initial publication of Semmelweis‘ findings. Meigs also objects Semmelweis (as «Semmelweiss»). His book is a compilation of examples to suggest doctors were not guilty of spreading puerperal fever. 1

Meigs explicitly rejects the implicit accusation of having himself been an agent of transmission of puerperal fever: «[…] I certainly was never the medium of its transmission.» 2

Unlike Semmelweis, Holmes did not let his adversaries take control of his life, made his point and moved on. Despite the opposition, his conclusions were known in Britain and, somewhat ironically, when Semmelweis stressed the importance of hand hygiene to prevent contagion with puerperal fever, his claims were rejected also under the pretense that they were not new.

Chloride of lime, which Semmelweis would advocate, was long believed to have disinfecting effect, including against puerperal fever, as is evidenced by an account from 1829 referenced by Meigs. 3

Autopsies were standard practice for post mortem diagnosis or to instruct medical students, which also indicates that medicine was busy moving towards scientific scrutiny.

And autopsies, Semmelweis revealed, were an essential part of the problem. After having tested and rejected at least two very different hypotheses, he identified a deadly cycle that killed about one hundred thousand women per year in obstetrics clinics —and would keep killing them for at least two more decades, despite Semmelweis‘ discovery.

It was common practice for students or doctors of obstetrics to examine the body of deceased. Without properly washing their hands in between, they could dissect the body of a woman who had fallen victim to puerperal fever and go on to examine the vaginas of pregnant women. With their fingers they recycled the disease from the dead to the living, sometimes to the yet unborn too.

Several factors helped Semmelweis identify the problem. The Vienna maternity clinic had two branches, one operated by doctors and students, the other mostly by midwifes. The pregnant women who were directed at the doctor’s branch of the clinic suffered and died from childbed disease significantly more often than those who were looked after by midwifes —who also refrained from vaginal inspections.

The two branches‘ different reputation made many women solicit to be permitted to the safer branch of the maternity and they sometimes cried in desperation if their wish was disregarded. (They were not given the choice.)

Another hint was the death of a pathologist, a dear colleague of Semmelweis. After having been wounded by a student’s scalpel during an autopsy, the pathologist contracted a fever and died. The symptoms of his disease and his dead body suspiciously resembled those of the women who all too regularly died in their childbed.

Last but not least, Semmelweis —unlike most of his colleagues— acknowledged that he himself was a major part of the problem.

A conflict with his superior, Johann Klein, helped him discover his own deadly effect. Semmelweis was not Klein’s protégé, to say the least. Perhaps, differences between the two sprang from Semmelweis‘ ambition as a scientist. Klein’s scientific ambitions flew low and he might have preferred to obstruct Semmelweis‘ career. Or, possibly, their opinions diverged over political issues. Whatever the reason was, in late 1846, Semmelweis‘ appointment was not prolonged and he, who had done most of the autopsies, had to pause.

As a consequence, deaths from puerperal fever in the men-run branch of the maternity clinic dropped to a few percent, the level of the branch run by midwifes. When, soon afterwards, in March 1847, Semmelweis was allowed back to work, autopsies resumed and death rates sprang back up to near record heights.

Semmelweis concluded and imposed that hands needed to be washed thoroughly in a chlorine solution between autopsies and vaginal inspections. Consequently, death rates from childbed fever plummeted. That was in May 1847, 18 years before Semmelweis‘ death.

Streptococcus pyogenes bacteria
The truly deadly agent was discovered after Semmelweis‘ death. | Scanning electron micrograph of Streptococcus pyogenes bacteria on primary human neutrophil. Credit: NIAID (Flickr)

He believed that traces of decayed material from the dissected bodies stuck to the fingers of the doctors and students. (Streptococcus pyogenes bacteria then infected the unfortunate women.)

Two years after his breakthrough at the hospital, his assignment was again not prolonged, definitely, this time. He was offered a minor position in academia but refrained from accepting it.

A German speaker from Buda, Semmelweis returned to Pest, disappointed. (The two parts of today’s Budapest were not yet united.) There he took an unpaid position as the honorary director of a minor maternity clinic. While the problem had been rampant before, under Semmelweis, puerperal fever was eliminated almost entirely in that clinic. But once again, he was disregarded, even vilified.

The story of success but disregard repeated itself once again, after he had been able to get a position at the university hospital in Pest.

He is now considered a pioneer of evidence-based medicine. However, several decades should pass between his achievements and the posthumous recognition of his work and his rehabilitation.

Why the rejection?

It is often claimed that he was ignored, treated with disrespect and even accused of wrongdoing because he attacked and offended his colleague doctors unnecessarily the longer they ignored his findings and recommendations. These claims are difficult to support, at least from his public communications. In his 102-page open letter to obstetricians throughout Europe, which he wrote in 1861, his tone is not extraordinarily offensive —compared to, for example, how Karl Marx, a contemporary of Semmelweis, wrote about his competitors in the same trade.

Rather, there is indication that Semmelweis sought to minimize the faults of his colleagues and weaken the accusation.

While Semmelweis claimed that all puerperal fever infections «from outside» could be eliminated without exception if strict hygiene were observed, he attributed residual cases of childbed fever not to transmission but to infection «from inside», as he called it. He claimed that a deadly material, essentially the same «decayed beastly-organic substance» («zersetzter thierisch-organischer Stoff») that he suspected were transferred from the sick or dead to the healthy and living, could also develop inside the victims rather than always having been brought about from the outside.

He suggested this way of «internal» infection even though there had been a long time at the Vienna hospital without any deaths from childbed fever, before autopsies were made. And there was an entire month without deaths from childbed fever, March 1848, when Semmelweis had temporarily succeeded to enforce a more rigorous hygiene policy: Doctors had to wash their hands not only after autopsies, but before vaginal inspections, too.

These periods without deaths essentially disproved the hypothesis of spontaneous internal development of the infection. It is hardly conceivable that this conclusion escaped Semmelweis. He nevertheless excused his colleagues from residual fatalities, when he probably should have attributed them to a lack of strict observation of his policy or generally inadequate levels of hygiene —the latter of which he also correctly believed were another way of disease transmission (open wounds, transmission via instruments, bed sheets, etc.).

Semmelweis was rather trying to find a way to excuse his colleagues than to accuse them for all fatalities from puerperal disease among their patients. (Semmelweis was nevertheless somewhat right with his suspicion of internal infection. The dangerous bacteria could be brought to the hospital by a pregnant woman with infected respiratory organs, but he could not have known about it.)

Either way, whether Semmelweis unnecessarily accused —as mainstream historic opinion posits— or whether he, as I found, had rather been seeking to avoid accusations: It is safe to say that for him the truth was more important than friendship and his social and professional environment. And, either way, whether implicit or explicit, his message war loaded with accusation.

Merely half a dozen authorities in his field supported Semmelweis. (One of them felt so deeply ashamed that he committed suicide after reckoning to have infected his pregnant cousin.) All other obstetricians ignored or rejected his findings, often vehemently. Yet, some of Semmelweis‘ most vocal critics, including his nemesis Carl Braun, discretely ruled out autopsies or vaginal inspections or strictly separated the two activities, with notable results, Semmelweis claimed. Many doctors knew Semmelweis was right, he pointed out in his open letter from 1861, but they did not admit it.

After having seen his achievements dismissed, Semmelweis remained rather silent. From 1858 onwards, however, he made another attempt to make his voice heard and published three books, including his main work, in 1861. It was also largely dismissed.

Naturally, it became gradually more difficult for Semmelweis not to appear being offensive towards his peers. At first the implicit message had been that they killed women inadvertently. As time went by and women kept dying, the implicit message inevitable became this: You keep on killing hundreds of thousands of women, knowingly. Semmelweis then also accused his colleagues explicitly, at least in closed letters.


Why is it so easy for the special interest groups working on behalf of the fossil fuel industry to make their voices heard and propagate denial, while climate communicators keep failing at their task?


Part 2 – Impact of Guilt Induction and Guilt Avoidance on Climate Communication

Reviewing the Semmelweis reflex

The «Semmelweis reflex» or «Semmelweis effect» is supposed to explain Semmelweis‘ failure to make his message heard. In Wikipedia it is «a metaphor for the reflex-like tendency to reject new evidence or new knowledge because it contradicts established norms, beliefs or paradigms».

However, this definition misses the main reason why Semmelweis failed to be persuasive. He had virtually had no choice but to accuse his colleagues. Even when he didn’t accuse them explicitly there was still an implicit but stark accusation, because the life of so many were obliterated.

Moreover, the accusation was linked to impurity, an accusation difficult for people to put up with psychologically. (It is almost a historical standard to vilify people by claiming them being impure; The «psychology of disgust and contamination» is considered to be an essential element of our moral foundation; Reticence towards hand hygiene in hospitals is still a big problem.)

There is a special element to Semmelweis‘ problem that goes beyond the biases that the current definition of the Semmelweis reflex suggests. (E.g. belief perseverance, an endowment effect for including non-material goods, the confirmation bias, the tendency to cling to an existing theory or worldview —and to reject a new theory or worldview, groupthink and belief in authority.)

This special element is the implicit accusation.

It would be sensible to redefine the Semmelweis reflex to incorporate the characteristic and probably determining aspect of Semmelweis‘ problem:

If a message, implicitly or explicitly, includes an accusation of the recipient, the latter is inclined to reject the message and has a tendency to accuse the messenger instead.

The rejection of the message and the tendency to raise a counter-accusation are two different elements of the suggested redefinition of the Semmelweis reflex. These two elements could be kept apart.

  • The rejection of a message with an underlying accusation could be called accusation rejection bias.
  • The tendency to accuse the messenger if the original message includes an implicit (or explicit) accusation could be called accusation reflection effect.

Semmelweis‘ problem was not so much the contradiction of «established norms», as the current definition of the Semmelweis reflex states. Much more, his problem was that, even though he did not say it like this at all, the message to his colleagues was inevitably heard like this: You kill women en masse by sticking your filthy fingers into their vaginas.

Who would want to perceive that? It is not surprising that Semmelweis‘ message was not well received.

The don’t kill the messenger saying proclaims the difficulties there are to convey an inconvenient truth. Semmelweis‘ message was far more troublesome than it was inconvenient. Even if implicit, it was a severe accusation. It is not surprising that his message was difficult to get across.

Distorted communication

There is another reason, a different reason, why Semmelweis gained little support. Not only did his colleagues not want to hear what he had found out, they did not want to tell it either. There is yet another psychological factor that aggravated Semmelweis‘ challenge.

It could be called implicit accusation inhibition to describe our reluctance to communicate fully or communicate at all, a tendency to communicate mildly or the failure to communicate correctly if the message includes an accusation, even if the accusation is implicit.

Psychologists found out long ago that humans rarely tell things as they are or that we are astonishingly reluctant to say what would have to be told in order to be honest. This is because the undisguised message is often not appealing to the people we communicate with.

We must always be worried about making friends and allies and not losing them. We therefore almost always carefully navigate between being honest to ourselves and the facts and avoid being offensive.

Not only our perception is full of biases that serve to please us. The active part of our communication is also biased. It is skewed to please the recipients of our messages in order not to disappoint or upset them.

The Semmelweis reflex as posited above, which stresses the element of accusation, and the implicit accusation inhibition are key to understanding important biases in climate communication.

Biased climate communication

There are various degrees of implicit accusation inhibition. In terms of climate change, they may be distinguished as follows:

  1. Climate silence (reluctance to communicate; not talk about global warming)
  2. Preference to downplay the importance of climate change, to discover and spread comforting, mild or positive climate information (lesser accusation messaging)
  3. Active climate denial (communicate incorrectly; actively question the existence of climate change, its causes or its effects)

These categories of the implicit accusation inhibition (A, B and C) are discussed separately in the following sections.

Climate silence (A)

Survey results for the United States should make us attentive. There is a strange silence on climate change. It is well documented and reported for the US. There is awareness of the problem and an organization which aims to counteract it. But, of course, the silence on climate change is not restricted to the US where people don’t want to talk about a problem they nevertheless want to solve (see video clip below).

«Americans believe they can solve a problem, even if they don’t believe we have a problem but they are not talking about it.» | Richard Alley in press meeting at AGU 2017 conference. Clip. Full video.

It has been tried to explain climate silence as the result of a spiral of silence, a mechanism related to groupthink, the tendency to align ones own opinion with mainstream opinion. To avoid isolation, those who do not adjust their opinion nevertheless keep quiet and thereby relatively strengthen the prevailing opinion, which causes a reinforcing feedback and elicits a spiral, the spiral of silence.

Groupthink, which is part of the spiral of silence theory, is a very important factor in climate communications and the perception of the problem, there can be not doubt about that. 4

The spiral of silence theory is compelling on its own and the theory is backed by observations. However, it falls short of explaining the silence among those who think climate change is real and should be addressed. And that is the majority, even in the US. Moreover, for the spiral of silence to start, there must be an initial inclination to reject the science of climate change, a widespread psychological inclination to not want to talk about it or an incentive not to talk about it.

There must be a different explanation for the observed silence, at least for its beginning. There is a psychological driver, a deeper cause for the silence. It is the implicit accusation inhibition.

We don’t want to talk about climate change with our neighbors, friends, relatives or peers —or voters (Germany, US 2012, US 2016)— because nobody wants to hear it, feel accused and guilty.

We neither want to accuse nor do we want to be accused. Even if there is no real accusation made, there is still fear that the climate message might be perceived as an accusation.

Astronomer and science communicator Harald Lesch alludes to the difficulty and reticence of communicating climate change because people just don’t want to hear about it. With his video answer a denier could hardly be more affirmative. At the beginning of a TV-broadcast to explain global warming, Harald Lesch asks: «How should I start?» The climate denier intercepts (blue panes): «Not at all! You already made a fool of yourself too much with your climate blabber and outed yourself as criminal climate liar.» «I keep repeating it, climate change is not an easy topic», continues Harald Lesch. The denier intercepts: «True, it is by now a repulsive, boring topic which JUST AND ONLY gets on people’s nerves because they can no longer stand hearing the rubbish and the lies!» Harald Lesch concludes: «In this way I began the broadcast and I said: ‚for heavens sake he starts [I start] talking about climate change again!‘, now here he also finishes his talk [I also finish my talk] with climate change.» «HOPEFULLY you finally finish with it and HOPEFULLY, you will be held responsible for your lying», intercepts the denier. | Compilation with beginning and end of a climate denier’s video based on a TV-broadcast by German ZDF.

The reluctance to talk to neighbors or friends about global warming is comprehensible. If we bring the topic up, we risk that their reply might be similar to this: «You also drive a car, fly, heat your home, use electricity or eat meat. Why do you talk to me about it?» People are unlikely to respond in this way. There is nevertheless much reason to believe they think in this way. And that is reason enough to keep quiet in the first place.

Intriguingly, Genevieve Gunther, who heads an organization, which is specifically devoted to end climate silence, is a climate preacher. In tweets she repeatedly criticized Leonardo DiCaprio for his flying. It is almost as if she were asking the superstar —one of too few to speak out on it— to shut up on climate change! Our urge to preach will be a topic in part 3.

Science is the method designed to elaborate the truth. We can expect communications by scientists to be more correct than average and I dare to claim that they usually are considerably more correct than average. However, scientists are also people with psychological biases. And even scientists cannot be perfectly correct.

Are there cases of implicit accusation inhibition or lesser accusation messaging in communications by scientists? There is reason to believe so. This will be the topic of the next section.

Lesser accusation messaging, scientific reticence and the least drama (B)

Implicit accusation inhibition may be the key reason for many climate scientists to be reticent to present inconvenient truths. This reticence was pointed out by James Hansen long ago. Climate scientists often err on the side of the least drama, other scientists explain.

A more recent case of lesser accusation messaging is the claim that we can allow for a lot more cumulative CO2 emissions than previously thought to stay within 1.5 degrees of warming.

To blame climate change on the sun is a classic of climate change denial. The rise of global temperature is clearly not due to the sun, as probably every climate scientist familiar with the topic would confirm. However, some scientists posit the sun might come to help in the future. It may be quality science, but —it remains to be seen—, it might be a case of lesser accusation messaging. Either way, of course, the deniers are happy to use the information to deceive the public (as is explained by Peter Hadfield). Earlier, but similarly, German denier Fritz Vahrenholt had received too much attention when he claimed the sun will help mitigate climate change.

A case of lesser accusation messaging has even gone mainstream among climate scientists. It is equally important as it is outrageous. Future generations, they assume or even suggest to rely on, will net remove massive amounts of CO2 from the atmosphere in the second half of this century (Joeri Rogelj here; UNEP there, page 50, tacitly cut-off at 2050 to hide the assumption of massive net carbon dioxide removal). As crazy as it may seem, this ill-fated assumption already serves to rate (underrate!) the compatibility (incompatibility!) of the countries‘ mitigation pledges with the temperature targets set in the Paris Agreement.

Almost ten years ago, climate scientists started to finally, finally, but still cautiously, explain to the world what they should have been shouting out loud for decades: That CO2 emission must be eliminated altogether —completely. They should, finally, tell the (not) ‚policy makers‘ that CO2 emissions must be cut now, right now, not later. But only a few reputed climate scientist dare to clearly hold this position and question the feasibility of massive net CO2 removal, as Stefan Rahmstorf does here. (As yet, nobody —as far as I can see— denounces the real Achilles heel of the belief in massive net CO2 removal, the missing political feasibility, the deficiency in global governance.)

The long reluctance to explain the need for a zero carbon economy and the claim that there will be significant net CO2 removal are both very consequential cases of implicit accusation inhibition — by omission or by misleading optimism, respectively.

Active climate denial as related to the implicit accusation inhibition (C)

There is a widely accepted taxonomy of global warming denial, established by John Cook and the team at Skeptical Science:

  • Level 1: It’s not happening
  • Level 2: It’s not us —or it isn’t CO2
  • Level 3: It’s not bad —or CO2 isn’t
  • Level 4: It’s too hard to solve

These levels differ in their degree of problem acknowledgement, but the avoidance of guilt is common to at least the first three levels of denial.

Consistent with the main argument of this section, among the very active deniers, the strongest rejection is on level 2: It’s not us —or it isn’t CO2! Climate denial often calls into question the effect of CO2, the greenhouse gas we all know we emit.

There is an extremely successful case of communication that removes guilt from the audience. Guilt removal is a central element of at least one world religion. The receptivity to messages that assert we are not guilty of causing climate change is not surprising. The temptation to suggest «not guilty!» or, a more adequate position for a scientist, to succumb to lesser accusation messaging, is huge.

Only very few true climate scientists reject the scientific evidence of human caused global warming. Typically, the opinion of the few deniers among the real climate scientists is that the human influence on the climate is smaller than what the overwhelming majority of climate scientists say. (At least the overwhelming part of the observed warming is man made; our best assessments suggest that all observed warming is anthropogenic or even slightly more than the observed warming.)

One of the few climate scientists who argue for a small human influence and a small rate of warming is John Christy (who declares to be a Christian). I believe that John Christy, as well as other scientists who question the human influence or consider global warming a minor problem, are driven by a desire not to feel guilty or are driven by a desire not to accuse their audience of being responsible for climate change.

You are not guilty, me neither

The preceding sections described how climate change is communicated in order to please the recipients of the messages and make the messaging psychologically palatable. However, by suggesting that the recipient is not guilty (or less guilty), the communicator also suggests not to be guilty (or less guilty) himself. It is difficult to say which psychological motivation, guilt removal from oneself or from the audience, is more important in a specific case. To try to deflect guilt from oneself is certainly an important contributor to distorted climate communication. It is discussed in later sections.

After having briefly treated climate silence, lesser accusation messaging and active denial, the following sections are about the receptive end of climate communication, the perception of the climate message and passive denial.

Induction of guilt and the perception of climate change

Almost everybody in the industrialized world drives a car, flies or buys stuff produced by means of fossil fuels. Virtually everybody contributes to CO2 emissions. The perception of the climate change message as an accusation is inevitable —at least to some extent—, even if no accusation is made explicitly.

«… running a bit hot!» Blatant accusation and self-centered impression management by nerdy activist. It rarely takes place between neighbors, and if the appeals are not very subtle, it quickly gets comical. | Video excerpt from Modern Family. Original)

Countless species will be driven to extinction by global warming and CO2 in the oceans, and that will be forever. For those worried more about human welfare, there is plenty of material to spot a strong accusation in the climate message, too. There is much reason to feel guilty.

There can be no doubt about the effectiveness and motivation of the professional ‚denial machine‘ with its sponsors in the fossil energy industry. However, both, the professional Merchants of Doubt and their sponsors might be driven not only by economic interest but a desire to avoid guilt by themselves and prevent accusations.

Despite the undeniable impact of deliberate deception by the fossil fuel special interests, this important question remains: Why is it so easy for the special interest groups working on behalf of the fossil fuel industry to make their voices heard and propagate denial, while climate communicators keep failing at their task?

It is because the public wants to hear that it is not guilty.

One important type of climate change communication, perhaps the dominating type of climate communication, was so far left aside: climate preaching, the stream of moral appeals to consume less or consume green or, more generally, the suggestion of voluntary personal carbon footprint reduction.

To approach the problem with climate preaching, let us first look at some examples of how the climate message, including the climate scientists‘ message, is received by the public.

A pattern in the rejection of climate science

There is a strange refusal to acknowledge the ‚human caused‘ part of the climate problem, but there is agreement to solve it. There is an easy explanation for this seeming paradox: We have plenty of reason to feel guilty about the cause of the problem. But nobody has a reason to feel guilty about its solutions.

Engineers should be particularly adept at understanding and appreciating science and physics. However, someone who did a lot to counter active climate contrarians once contemplated: «A typical denier is an engineer in his fifties.» Scientists confirm his observation. What engineers do in their professional lives almost always results in important CO2 emissions. Consequently, engineers have more reason than average to pretend not to be guilty. By denying the problem they avoid feeling guilty and try not to be seen as guilty.

The United States is the country of gas guzzling cars and super-consumption; the country with rampant per capita CO2 emissions. It is also the country of individualism, where government is vilified or marginalized, where individual responsibility is overstated and overrated. Climate denial is rampant in the US, too. The US as a nation and the average US consumer has exceptionally good reason to reject a feeling of guilt. Firstly, US emissions are particularly high. Secondly, American culture, like no other, posits individual responsibility, including for global warming. Consequently the individual feels the accusation and avoids it by denying the problem.

On the other side, people in countries that don’t much cause it, but rather suffer from global warming, are inclined to accept that climate change exists and that it is caused by human activity. Sometimes they even blame climate change to be at work where it isn’t. It is no coincidence that their perception of the problem is opposed to the perception of those who live in countries with high carbon emissions. They have the least reason to feel accused and guilty.

Nobody wants to feel guilty. One way to avoid a feeling of guilt is to deny the problem altogether. If global warming is acknowledged, it should not be CO2 (because that would mean me).

Intriguingly, the same psychology is at work with many climate activists who believe in self-centered action on climate change.

These activists almost systematically overestimate the role of methane as a greenhouse gas, compared to CO2. For any city dweller it appears to be an attractive proposition because, if methane is a main climate driver, cows or farmers are to blame. Furthermore, animal rights activists strongly influence the climate debate. Those who eat meat are at fault —not us. Have you ever wondered why vegetarians and vegans like the methane argument so much? To inflate the personal contribution they make by voluntarily abstaining from eating meat they distort the facts much like the sleekest and most stubborn of the climate deniers. The parallels between those who exaggerate their personal contribution (including with climate preaching) and the deniers are puzzling. The same psychology is at work with both groups and —one argument of this article—, the same emotion seems to be at work: guilt or the avoidance of it. 5

If you are often in touch with people who are environmentally concerned or even engaged in environmental action, including climate action, you notice something strange: Sometimes even they disbelieve that global warming is happening or that it’s caused by human activity. Can it be explained? I believe it can. There are essentially two ways to deal with guilt on climate change: Act or deny. Many environmentalists, I dare to hypothesize, are particularly susceptible to experience guilt, or particularly uncomfortable with the feeling, and use both methods to do away with the unwanted feeling.

Among environmentalists, there is widespread belief that forests remove CO2 from the atmosphere. They don’t. (Only growing forests do. But the potential of growing forests to undo our fossil fuel based emissions is far too limited, which should not be difficult to understand.) Many environmentalists nevertheless want to believe that, with time, existing forests will eventually correct our CO2 emissions altogether or they overestimate the potential of new forests to sequester and store carbon. A safe way to unequivocally be considered a climate hero is to start a tree planting initiative, although more forests don’t unequivocally cool the planet. (They do in the tropics but don’t in the Arctic, because forests alter the albedo.) The belief that trees will naturally —almost magically— undo what we do with fossil fuels comforts the mind.

A strange pattern of preaching and denial

An article by Michèle Binswanger with the title She leaves climate deniers fuming about Greta Thunberg, who broke ground with her climate strike, provoked more than 600 comments, most of them from deniers —as the article’s title proclaimed. The deniers express the motive for their denial. It is the reproach upon them that they emit CO2. «What did she do», one of them wrote, «fly to Poland to tell others that they produce too much CO2?» Another commentator opposed the deniers and pointed out that Greta travelled by electric car. Challenging the deniers, he added: «But it is always easier to criticize others than to do something yourself.» The deniers then calculated and posted the indirect CO2-emissions associated with Greta’s road trip to the UN climate conference where she spoke. (Why are the deniers so often worried about CO2 emissions, when these emissions are not a problem? But things get even stranger.) A denier responded:

«My current lifestyle and travel habits are probably almost exemplary in terms of CO2-savings for Swiss standards, I therefore have no reason to „criticize but not change“. Who just wasted energy to travel hundreds of kilometers to criticize others? My word: The girl. I hope you NEVER fly and live close to were you work, only use public transports or your own feet. If not, you’ll have much to catch up on.»

There is no reason for a denier to live a low-carbon lifestyle, but, as unreasonable as it may seem, deniers pointing out their own small carbon footprint abound. Actually, in the quote above, the climate denier is a climate preacher! He is not an exception.

There are many active deniers who are also climate activists, insofar as they limit their personal carbon footprint and brag about it. And there are many climate activists and preachers, including very active ones, who are also climate deniers. (I don’t know what you think about this, dear reader, but I think these two observations alone should make all bells ring loud and clear with everybody who seeks to understand climate denial.) In a way —think of it—, they are the same group. Anyway, they are motivated by the same psychology. That psychology serves to avoid a feeling of guilt and it serves the same ultimate purpose: dodge the work involved in engaging in real climate action to solve the problem. Could both, self-centered consumption-level climate activism and denial, be spurred by the same pieces of communication, climate preaching? In any case, the parallels are striking.

Before moving on to climate preaching and its guilt-inducing effect, we shall compare some other cases of science denial with the denial of global warming —in addition to Ignaz Semmelweis and puerperal fever from part one. There are more parallels to be drawn.

The parallel almost too terrible to mention

The Holocaust may not normally be compared to anything else. Its outrageousness demands exclusivity. In the Swiss parliament, Jonas Fricker recently compared the transportation of pigs to be killed in slaughterhouses with the death trains of the Nazis. He saw himself quickly politically lynched, including by members of his own party, the Greens, assisted by one popular newspaper (despite alleged regret, in the same bed). This happened although Fricker has no affinity with those inclined to belittle or deny the severity of the crime that the industrial killing on Nazi territory was. Within days of his controversial statement, Fricker resigned from his post as a member of parliament.

It may seem to be unrelated and, true, it is a little deviation, but, since I am here, it makes sense to report on it: On November 14, 2018 Jonas Fricker posted on social media: «Today, I start my personal experiment ‚finishing up with consumerism‘: From today onwards, for one year, I won’t buy any personal consumer article (…) Who joins in?» Another one is moving from political action to self centered action. Maybe, I should start a file to keep counting.

Journalist Peter Hadfield, who, as Potholer54, debunks climate denial like no other, abstains from calling climate deniers what they are («deniers»), because he deems the term to allude too strongly to Holocaust denier and doesn’t want the two kinds of deniers to be compared, associated or confused with one another.

It should not be necessary to state it. This section in no way aims at belittling the Holocaust committed under Nazi rule.

While the severity of the Holocaust should not be under debate, the severity of climate change lies mostly in the future and therefore remains to be experienced and judged about. To refrain from any comparison of elements of the Holocaust with elements of what is being done now would be wrong. For example, Adolf Hitler never visited a concentration camp. How did and does Donald Trump deal with the people of Puerto Rico before and after hurricane Maria? There is a huge difference between what was done under Hitler and what is done, or rather, is omitted, in the US under the current administration. However, these differences should not forbid the drawing of parallels between aspects of the Holocaust and aspects of other topics.

Another example: The complacency of high level politics and the general public in the face of global warming is reminiscent of the complacency by those who knew and should have known among the Nazi rulers and those under the Nazi regime —although, again, there are very important differences.

While climate change and the industrial killing in concentration camps are clearly not the same thing —that should go without saying—, there are parallels in the denial of climate change and the denial of the Holocaust, respectively.

Both cases of denial are supposedly improbable. They are both extremely strange. Both cases of denial should very clearly not be there.

The peculiarity of the denial of the Nazi concentration camps and the mass killings should not require much explanation. There are preserved camps, victims, survivors, liberators and other witnesses, interrogation protocols, testimonies, files with names. There are films and photographs, to mention the probably most amazing pieces of proof.

Keeling Curve. Mauna Loa CO2 measurements.
It was largely believed that CO2 emissions would be absorbed by the ocean, a belief consistent with desirability and implicit accusation inhibition. But CO2 accumulates in the atmosphere. | Keeling Curve.

The peculiarity of the denial of global warming takes slightly more space to explain. The greenhouse effect of atmospheric gases was first theorized by Joseph Fourier in 1824. 36 years later, in 1860, John Tyndall measured the capacity of some gases in the atmosphere to absorb and emit long wave electromagnetic radiation. Only during these 36 years, more than 150 years ago, the atmospheric greenhouse effect and its consequence for earth’s surface temperature was truly a theory. In the 19th century, however, only few specialists were interested in the issue and the public abstained from the debate. If there was any debate at all, it took place a long time ago. When the greenhouse effect of the atmosphere became an acknowledged fact and ceased to be a theory, the public did not take notice.

When the changing climate due to its human cause was recognized —at least as a prediction based on emissions trajectories— the implicit accusation inhibition soon impacted the debate and temporary caveats against the warming effect of CO2-emissions appeared —consistent with the hypothesis of lesser accusation messaging.

However, these caveats were rather quickly moved out of the way (for example by Guy Callendar or Charles Keeling). Because there is only scant public awareness of the early transition global warming «theory» made from hypothesis to acknowledged fact, the fossil fuel industry succeeded in relegating it in the public domain to the level of a «theory».

If the evidence is as solid as it is for climate change or the Holocaust, there is not normally any denial anymore. This will become clear from the comparison with a historical, exemplary case of science denial.

Continental drift

For decades, there was a fiercely fought scientific debate over the existence, or not, of continental drift. The public took notice and participated in the debate. Evidence accumulated to the extent that the theory could at some point virtually be considered proven. In the opposing camp, many scientists and much of the public nevertheless clung to their old conviction and rejected continental drift theory.

However, when it was discovered that ocean crust is formed continually and new ocean crust displaces older ocean crust, which in turn shifts continental plates around, the debate over continental drift was terminated. Scientists now measure the amount of continental drift, which confirms and quantifies the effect of the discovered mechanism at work. These days, you don’t notice many still insisting that continental plates are immobile.

When there is proof provided for a theory, when the physics are revealed and understood, there is no longer any debate and the theory becomes an acknowledged fact. Unequivocal results from reproducible measurements that provide proof of a theory normally terminate any debate around a scientific issue. At least, that is what could be expected. Not so with the denial of the Holocaust, though. Not so with global warming denial, either.

These days, many scientists measure the warming of planet earth, which confirms the effect of the measured radiation properties and measured elevated concentrations of greenhouse gases. The temperature evolves in congruence with proven physics. Both cause and effect are evidenced by measurements, as is the case with plate tectonics.

The denial of global warming is not just a bit weird. It is excessively strange. Why was the debate settled in one case, plate tectonics, but not in the other two cases? Humans do not cause continental drift. In the other cases, humans were or are the cause. The difference is the negative human involvement, the element of guilt.

If it is asserted that guilt rejection favors the denial of the Holocaust, it may be objected that its deniers were not responsible for the wrongdoing themselves, which is of course true. Their parents were responsible or their grandparents, or the former’s or the latter’s friends or compatriots, or someone else with the same or a similar political orientation —or just another human being did it or failed to counteract the crime. It doesn’t have to be precisely guilt, or felt shame for oneself, that causes denial. It may be anything that negatively impacts on one’s own self-esteem through negatively impacting on human self-esteem.

How far back in time can the wrongdoing be or how much dispersed in a large group can the responsibility be to negatively affect self-esteem or induce a feeling of discomfort, guilt or shame —and denial? We can look at another case of science denial to find the answer to that question.

Quaternary extinctions

Whenever the capable, well-armed and cooperative hunters of the species Homo sapiens found or conquered new land, they drove many animal species to extinction. Eurasia, the Americas, the many islands of the Pacific (every one of them), the islands of the Siberian arctic ocean, what are today New Zealand or Madagascar: Whichever scene you chose: Sapiens arrived. Animal species disappeared.

The later the invasion, the more skilled and better armed the hunters, the more thorough were the extinctions. Most manifestly affected was the megafauna, i.e. animals about the size of humans or larger. The tragedy —another tragedy of the commons— is sometimes referred to as the Quaternary extinctions.

True, nobody was there to photograph the slaughtering or count animals. Nobody was there to report on snow cover, precipitation or sunspots, etc., either. The evidence for the Quaternary extinctions is nevertheless overwhelming. But not only the evidence is rampant. Still, there is widespread denial of the Quaternary extinctions, even among scientists, but certainly in the public sphere.

At least two other hominid species (Neanderthals and Denisovans) were among the victims of Sapiens. This provides additional psychological reason to repress the facts on the Quaternary extinctions.

Many seriously proclaim that Sapiens mixed peacefully with Neanderthals or Denisovans. The remains of genes from our closest known relatives in the human genome suggest it, they point out. However, the small fraction of Neanderthal and Denisovan genes in humans really suggests otherwise —together with what historians and anthropologists know about territorial disputes and its outcome, rape associated with warfare or men violently raiding for women.

As far as non-human species are concerned, not the extinctions themselves are still being questioned. The human cause of it is! (Does that sound familiar?) To explain the Quaternary extinctions, climate change is invented over and over again, no matter how little plausible that theory is.

When it helps our self-esteem, climatic change was there. When it damages our self-esteem, climate change isn’t there. The human mind, its creativity and its capacity to be biased, is remarkably flexible. It is capable of making things up if it pleases its self-esteem. Not less impressive is the human mind’s capability to repress and distort facts if doing so comforts the mind and reassures the soul.

Models of Wooly mammoth and American mastodon.
Models of two of the best known of the many victims of the Late Quaternary Extinctions: A wooly mammoth (left) and an American mastodon.

There can be little doubt, that the denial of Darwinian evolution is dominated by religious concepts and early indoctrination. Undoubtedly, ideology plays a part in climate denial, too, as is succinctly explained by Eugenie Scott —but put in perspective by recent research. Additionally, the denial of evolution might be reinforced because it negatively impacts human self-esteem, too.

Climate preaching impacts negatively on one group of the population who interprets the preaching as an accusation, feels offended and rejects it by denying the problem —and safeguards its self-esteem. Another group welcomes and follows the preaching superficially and also preserves its self-esteem.

Although not the same thing, for our purpose, it is practically impossible to distinguish between the avoidance of guilt and the preservation of self-esteem. An accusation, whether consciously perceived or subconsciously received, inevitably impacts on ones self-esteem. In the place of guilt, self-esteem could have been the central expression in this article.

It would be far beyond this article’s scope to try and cover all underrepresented issues in the field. But there are more problems related to climate denial and self-esteem and it is fitting to mention some here.

More on self-esteem

Electronic media and the media’s principled belief in balance permit would-be experts to communicate with real experts and even have public debates. It provides deniers with opportunities for impression management to enhance their self-esteem.

Related and more important in the context of this article: When climate scientists get much respect and attention for working on climate change and for communicating on it, other people, particularly other scientists and would-be scientists, have a psychological motivation to discredit the real climate scientists‘ work. It may be seen as a matter of envy. Or it may be seen as a matter of self-esteem. If I believe that someone who is smarter than I, or more successful than I, is wrong, my belief helps me to safeguard my self-esteem.

I think many prominent climate deniers like Richard Lindzen or Bjørn Lomborg may be affected by this bias. If the hypothesis is true, it may —among other things— explain the surprising abundance of climate deniers from other sciences, including sciences close to climate science, like geology or meteorology, who missed the train to work on global warming and to take the right side on it.

This hypothesis may also explain the surprisingly tenacious resistance against new scientific theories in general, like continental drift. Envy can be a positive driver in the scientific process if it results in ambition. But if envy results in condescendence or even vilification it is not a productive element in that process.

As with Ignaz Semmelweis, there would be a story to be told to illustrate the hypothesis. In brief: From about 1890 onwards, German physicist Philipp Lenard worked very successfully on cathode rays. In 1905 he was awarded the Nobel Prize for his work. This work and the apparatus he conceived were essential for the discovery of the electron and X-rays. However, disappointingly for Lenard, other scientists, Joseph John Tomson and Conrad Röntgen, respectively, were credited above him for the two discoveries. From 1900 on, Lenard worked on the photoelectric effect, again with great achievements. But, once again, another scientist would be awarded the greatest honors. Albert Einstein was given the 1921 Nobel Prize in physics, not for his outstanding masterpiece, relativity, but for explaining the photoelectric effect based on Planck’s quantum physics. Planck, received the Nobel Prize for 2018. This did not bode well with Lenard’s self-esteem. He, and other scientists spent much time and effort to reject Einstein’s theories and achievements, as well as Planck’s.

The rejection of relativity in the Weimar Republic and Nazi Germany shares many traits with today’s denial of global warming, including ideology and the assertion of a conspiracy. Particularly in the case of Lenard’s denial of Einstein’s and Planck’s achievements, personal level competition and envy might have been at work.

Clive Hamilton pointed out in 2010 that climate change is rejected because it originates from the wrong side of the political spectrum, i.e. the other side. More recent research supports this view (Kahan, Lewis et al.).

But people accept or reject ideas and beliefs not only based on ideology and groupthink. They also reject them when they originate from what they consider the wrong person. For example, I know of people who reject everything proposed and discovered by William Nordhaus because they dislike this or that other piece of information originating from the Nobel Prize winning economist. Such a reaction is wrong and unhelpful. However, it is part of human nature.

As far as I know, although it is important, psychologists have so far not named this distortion of perception that could be called wrong-person bias. There is also the reverse analog, a right-person bias —paramount in commercial product marketing.

We climate communicators should avoid the trap of becoming the wrong person, when it can be avoided. We should try to communicate honestly, without exaggeration or deception. The claim that we could and should solve the climate crisis with individual voluntary consumer action is wrong and, I shall point out later, it may be perceived as a deception or manipulation.

For Philipp Lenard, relativity was not just wrong. It came from the wrong person, Albert Einstein, who won the big prize for having explained what he, Lenard, had failed to explain. And it came from a Jew. Lenard was among the first scientists to join the National Socialist party, at a time when the motivation for this move was likely ideology, not opportunism or necessity. Lenard’s arguments were often anti-Semitic —which, comfortably or uncomfortably, together with the observation that it is often difficult to keep motives apart, brings us back to the topic this section began with.

It may be guessed that Holocaust deniers are statistically inclined to belief in a just world. In view of the extreme injustice that the Holocaust was, they might prefer to deny the facts rather than revise their view of the world as a just one. The human psyche is complicated. Several biases may be at work to distort a piece of cognition.

A special bias for denial

The bias called just-world belief describes the human tendency to view the world as fairer than it actually is. This bias makes us believe, disproportionately, that people deserve what happens to them. There is a test to assess the degree to which selected people (i.e. participants of a psychological experiment) believe in a just world.

In 2010 scientists Matthew Feinberg and Robb Willer reported on two studies they conducted to determine specifically the influence of just-world belief on the denial of climate change. In their first study, participants who were told that climate change is practically unresolvable showed increased levels of climate denial, but only if they were strongly inclined to believe in a just world. If told that climate change can be resolved easily, denial in that same group was reduced almost equally significantly. Those on the other side of the spectrum of just-world belief were less affected by the messaging. They responded on both types of messaging with slightly but significantly reduced changes in their level of denial.

In the second study, one segment of the participants was primed with statements supporting or eliciting just-world belief. The complementary segment was primed with messages contrary to just-world belief. Subsequently both segments were confronted with dire statements about climate change, including one that elicited dramatically that innocent children would be hit. Finally, as in the first study, the participant’s level of denial of climate change was assessed. «Participants who were primed with just-world statements reported higher levels of global warming skepticism (…) than did those who were primed with unjust-world statements (…)», the scientists reported.

The combination of the studies lets the authors conclude that «dire messages warning of the severity of global warming and its presumed dangers can backfire, paradoxically increasing skepticism about global warming by contradicting individual’s deeply held beliefs that the world is fundamentally just.» (Feinberg and Willer, 2010)

A byproduct of the second study also deserves to be acknowledged. As part of the assessment of the level of denial, seven questions were asked, e.g. «How solid is the evidence that the earth is warming?» One of the questions was considerably different from the others: «Overall, how willing are you to change your current lifestyle in order to reduce your carbon footprint?» It is problematic to qualify a refusal to reduce one’s personal carbon footprint as climate denial. Fortunately, the researchers also thought like that, at least to some extent, and specifically focused on the pattern of replies to this question with respect to the other questions probing denial. They found that those primed with just-world belief refused to (state to be ready to) reduce their carbon footprint and that this reaction was fully mediated for by the increased induced level of denial through the induction of just-world belief.

What, I dare to ask, if the low effect of climate preaching on the voluntary reduction of personal CO2-emissions could be explained by (fully mediated by, fully caused by, fully compensated by) the preaching’s induction of guilt and the rejection of it? What if the small acts of voluntary reductions by a small segment of the population due to climate preaching were entirely compensated for by less voluntary action in the other segments of the population because the preaching induces guilt and enhances the refusal to voluntarily make greener choices?

I do not claim that appeals for self-centered voluntary climate action, like personal carbon footprint reduction, negatively impact self-centered voluntary action itself, on average over all segments of a population. However, in the light of Feinberg and Willer’s results, even that possibility should not a priori be discarded. (This article, specifically its part 4, claims that climate preaching hampers rather than fosters progress on climate change overall, not just in the domain of voluntary consumer action.)

Feinberg and Willer studied the combination of dire messaging, just-world belief and denial, which is not the same combination as climate preaching, guilt induction and denial. However, it would be difficult to overlook the similarities. Belief in a just world and a desire not to feel guilty are connected via desirability or the respective bias.

There are more parallels between the studies by Feinberg and Willer and arguments made in this piece.

  • First, the results produced by Feinberg and Willer show that the impact of climate messaging on climate denial may be very different for different groups.
  • Second, the design of the two studies permitted to target the influence of specifically the just-world belief bias. However, the bias might, like so many other biases, be related to self-esteem because a just world, or an unjust world, is made what it is mostly by human activity —as a different climate is now made by human activity.
  • Third, the belief in a just-world bias serves to justify free riding: There is no need to make the world a just place if it already is a just place. Climate change denial also serves as an excuse for inaction, i.e. free riding. (Actually, viewing beyond psychology, that is what climate change denial is all about.)
  • Fourth, one departure point of Feinberg and Willer’s enquiry was this question: «But what if these appeals are in fact counterproductive?» The readiness to question the usefulness of habitual guilt-inducing climate communications was a precondition for the studies and their success.
  • Fifth, but not least, dire messaging induces guilt. It augmented denial in one group but not in the other. Climate preaching induces guilt too. It augments denial in one group but not in the other, so it is argued in this article. Exposure to a climate message that portrayed global warming as a solvable problem —thus, less guilt induction or rather the opposite of guilt induction— decreased the level of denial regardless of the participants‘ belief in a just world («Positive Message» in the diagram below).
Figure 1 from Feinberg and Willer: Apocalypse Soon? Dire Messages Reduce Belief in Global Warming by Contradicting Just-World Beliefs. Matthew Feinberg and Robb Willer (2010), with original description.
Figure 1 from Feinberg and Willer (2010), with original description.

If there is distortion of cognition, one or more cognitive biases are at work. Many biases have in common that they play out if perception negatively impacts self-esteem. For this and other reasons, it is often difficult to keep the influence of various biases apart.

Moreover, feelings interplay with cognition. For example, it is difficult to imagine a message that evokes fear on climate change but does not also elicit guilt. And it would be difficult to quantify whether denial induced by such a message serves to avoid guilt or to repress fear.

The double bladed sword of fear induction

There can be little doubt that feeling fear has a superb potential to induce action. And it was discovered that fear makes us open minded insofar as it increases our readiness to consider varying pieces of information —which might help to counter denial. That is on one hand. On the other hand, it is almost ancient wisdom that fear can induce near total repression of danger in an individual if the cost of action is high or if the problem is believed to be bigger than the individuals capacity to resolve it. Not surprisingly, current wisdom on fear messaging on climate change can superficially be summarized like this: Its impact is rather negative, unless there is also hope as well as proximity, i.e. a perception of being immediately and personally at risk.

Practically, to elicit fear and hope at the same time is not easy. Fear-inducing messages tend to be dire and dire messaging discards hope, almost by definition.

Fear also increases social attitudes associated with the political right —or rather: security increases social attitudes associated with the political left. This was recently discovered by Jaime Napier and colleagues (Napier et al. 2018). And the political left rather accepts the science on climate change, while the political right rather denies it —albeit only very much so in western countries, according to another study.

Despite the complexity of emotional climate communications, and specifically fear induction, the debate over its up- and downsides progresses in the scientific domain. But there remains much confusion about it in the public and the media, as is explained by Lucia Graves in an article about the fear-piece by David Wallace-Wells in the New York Magazine, which «soon was the best-read story in the magazine’s history». Fear messaging about global warming, even if it is between implausible and absurd, is about as eagerly welcomed as is climate preaching.

Fear messaging and climate preaching also have in common that they suggest guilt and consequently induce rejection and denial in some people but may motivate others into action.

There is reason to believe that climate preaching induces denial more effectively than fear messaging, because climate preaching suggests more guilt, I suppose. There is reason to further speculate that fear messaging induces collective action, while climate preaching induces self-centered action —alas, that is what climate preaching aims at.

The study gap on guilt induction

One aim of this article is that the two-sided sword of guilt induction also gains the attention it deserves. But that would require putting aside the widespread a priori held belief that climate preaching is a good thing to do and should be well received.

My guess would be that people who are statistically much inclined to feel guilty about something (or people subjected to guilt induction), are either more likely than average to deny climate change (one segment of the population) or are more likely than average to respond positively to appeals for voluntary individual climate action (another segment) or do both: deny and respond positively to climate preaching. In contrast, I would expect those who are only weakly inclined to feel guilty (or were not made feel guilty through deliberate induction) to be only moderately inclined to deny climate change and moderately inclined to respond positively to climate preaching. In other words, I would expect climate preaching to cause denial and positive response, but more so among those who are inclined to feel guilty (or are made feel guilty as part of a study).

There may be other psychological factors or psychological predispositions which impact on denial as a consequence of climate preaching. Obvious candidates are: inclination to accept moral appeals; inclination to see them as an accusation; inclination to reject accusations; tendency to project one’s own guilt onto others.

With respect to the «inclination to accept moral appeals» I guess deniers have, more than average, affinity for moral standards (and maybe less affinity for legal standards). Without the inclination for moral standards, the appeals would rather be ignored than rejected, I suppose. It seems not to make sense but in a way it does. It would be interesting to know the opinion of trained theoretical psychologists on this speculation. I deduce the guess from statements in deniers‘ blogs.

Unlike guilt induction, accusation rejection and the related psychological predispositions, several more known biases are often discussed in the context of climate denial. How some of them relate to the hypotheses raised in this article will be discussed in the next section.

Confirmation and desirability biases

Confirmation bias makes people seek and readily accept information that supports their preexisting views, but overlook or reject contradicting pieces of information. There can be no doubt that this bias is very important in the field of climate change perception and denial. It explains the information bubble that the deniers have created and source from. It explains the ongoing polarization with a camp opposing the deniers, the climate doomers, who are also driven by the confirmation bias and have created their own bubble.

Like all biases, the confirmation bias serves a purpose. Without it, Albert Einstein would not have found out relativity. It seems to be awkward, but, giving it some thought, it is not surprising, that relatively intelligent people are particularly susceptible to the confirmation bias, as is explained in a short talk by Tali Sharot.

It is important to acknowledge, though, that the confirmation bias is not possibly the source bias that causes climate denial. There must be a desire to deny the facts on climate change in the first place. Only subsequently will the confirmation bias do its work. In the same talk, Tali Sharot also explains that desired, mind-comforting information is more easily accepted than discomforting information —an observation perfectly in line with the arguments of this article. This distortion of cognition is called desirability bias.

After looking at several seemingly improbable but real cases of denial, several biases favoring denial and a first piece of scientific work that supports the claims made in this article, I suggest to look at a single case of a seemingly improbable denier.

A science loving denier and his protesting ‚Conscience‘

The «improbable denier» is the notable zoologist, science communicator, journalist and author Matt Ridley. His biography and credentials speak for themselves —and they speak for him. I recommend his early books on science (The Red Queen; The Origins of Virtue) rather than his later books that are leaning towards opinion. In his well-known and controversial book The Rational Optimist he argues that humanity was and will be able to solve its challenges, essentially alone by human ingenuity, trade and technological progress. (However, although Matt Ridley believes that humanity will be able to deal with it rather easily, he considers climate change an optimist’s challenge —together with the development of Africa.)

While he accepts the fundamental facts on climate change, some of Matt Ridley’s arguments and his approach are habitual for climate denial: Sourcing from and referring to other deniers, cherry picking, rejecting established climate science and refuting climate scientists, i.e. the specialists who do know better, as is explained here.

Like other climate deniers, Matt Ridley does not appreciate being graded a denier, but I am not the only one to do it. However, Matt Ridley cannot be placed in any of two important categories of deniers. He is neither one of those driven by a combination of psychological biases and an inclination towards scientific ignorance, nor would I locate him among the well informed driven by money from the fossil fuel industry.

Someone who has devoted most of his life to science and is prized for reporting science is a very unlikely denier of climate change. Compare Matt Ridley’s denial with the position of other science communicators like Bill Nye (for example here and here) or Neil deGrasse Tyson (for example here and here) or Harald Lesch.

A biologist is also an unlikely denier, given the threat to the natural living world that climate change poses. Furthermore, Matt Ridley embraces sociobiology, which —my appreciation— is among the branches of science least compatible with cognitive biases. Biologist Edward O. Wilson’s view of climate change is rather opposed to Matt Ridley’s. E.O. Wilson is the «father» of sociobiology.

His ‚Conscience‘: «Dear Matt, we need to talk!»

Why, against all odds, is Matt Ridley a climate denier? On Viscount Ridley’s property there is Britain’s largest open cast coalmine. «From time to time, I stand accused of letting the fact that I have a commercial interest in coal, which I have declared many times and hereby do again, influence my assessment of climate science», Ridley states in this presentation in which he rejects being a denier while demonstrating (Dana Nuticelli) he is a kind of denier.

If Matt Ridley’s position on climate change is not primarily driven by material interest (which can reasonably be asserted), it makes perfect sense that the coal mining protesters referring to themselves as Matt Ridley’s Conscience suggest a bad conscience, i.e. a subtle feeling of guilt —or the avoidance of it—, being at the heart of his denial.

The two convenient untruths

Matt Ridley claims that climate change is no big deal, that not much must be done about it and, if anything were needed to be done, it will happen essentially all by itself because markets and progress will take care of it. That is convenient and desirable, very good news, indeed. If I accept it, I may go on with business as usual: No policy change is needed and no need for hard work towards policy change is required. No consequences of policy change must be put up with either.

It may be a bit less desirable but it is still very convenient: Essentially, climate preaching is good news too. All I have to do is see myself as a good doer for not flying to Bali, but only to, say, the Maldive Islands or taking trains, not planes —at least occasionally; for not driving a Porsche Cayenne but, say, a mid-sized Citroën, or a Prius, or a bicycle —at least occasionally; for not eating beef, only chicken, or fish, but trout not tuna, or no meat at all —at least occasionally, etc. If I fall for it, I may also go on with essentially business as usual: No policy change is needed and no need for hard work towards policy change is required. All that is required is a bit of green consumption, ideally paired with self-righteousness, with pretending to myself and, as a welcome side effect, also pretending to those around me.

Both, active climate denial and climate preaching, fall on fertile ground because both profit from the desirability bias. Actually, from the perspective of the individual, both a response to climate preaching and a response to active denial are desirable responses insofar as they provide a benefit for the individual who believes the message (denial) or follows the advice and commits little superficial acts of greener living. Both positions serve as excuses for not taking societally more relevant and more strenuous action.

There are more excuses. In the next sections, we shall have a closer look at another excuse for political apathy on climate change.

Despair as another convenient untruth

In his movie The Inconvenient Truth from 2006, Al Gore pointed out that people often go straight from denial to desperation, without pausing in the middle to take action. I have seen people transition from denial to alleged inevitable doom within less than one minute, which reveals to what degree their desperation, the claim that we can’t do anything about climate change, is just a comfortable assertion.

To assert or believe that nothing can be done to stop climate change has essentially the same purpose as the habitual superficial personal consumer action or denial: political passivity, political apathy. (Paradoxically, Al Gore’s The Inconvenient Truth concludes with [as usual, guess what?] … climate preaching. It would be interesting to know whether climate preaching also contributes to —alleged— desperation.)

When I claim there to be common roots for denial and desperation, I seem to be in good company. Prized climate scientist and communicator Stefan Rahmstorf recently commented on social media: «Desperation is the new denial.»

The climate doomists err on the side of the most drama —examples one, two. They are not usually climate scientists and —like many deniers— they are attention-grabbers. Their communication unnecessarily induces guilt and should be condemned.

When one asks whether the planet should be saved urgently I say: No it is not urgent, it is too late. Well, I’m radical in this respect. […] I say bye to the world. […] The problem is that we expect everything from governments. […] My mission is to tell the public: Let’s take back our individual responsibility, let’s think about our own acts. We can choose not to buy food wrapped in plastic, we can choose to travel less, one can choose to consider ones own carbon footprint. We could certainly protest to change our government’s acts but before and above all else, let’s consider what we do from morning to evening. […] We must make ourselves responsible. We are victims of the trap of consumption. […] We must think of our acts from getting up in the morning to the moment of taking to the bed. […] It is this kind of assuming our own individual responsibility that I fight for. | French actor Lambert Wilson in flash talk for france.tv. (Link)

It makes no sense to proclaim doom and to preach, but doomists are also often preachers (video above). The closeness and even personal overlap of the doomers, preachers and those who appreciate climate preaching would deserve an entire article or study. In any case, this closeness is another reason to see climate preaching critically.

While both doomism and denial serve to justify climate apathy and self-centered lifestyle adaption might serve to justify political apathy on climate change, there is outright activism by both climate doomists and, even more so, by deniers to communicate their position. This activism is the topic of the next section.

The (missing) economic case for active climate denial

Those who spearhead climate denial are often funded by the fossil energy industry.

However, first, even the professional deniers may be driven by the implicit accusation inhibition. Second, undoubtedly, most climate deniers, including many extremely zealous and active deniers, are not getting any money from big coal, oil or gas.

These deniers see themselves as Robin Hoods of truth fighting against the «conspiracy», the «hoax», «lies» and «deception» which the deniers believe to be purported by what they consider «climate alarmists». «Cui bono» (who profits), the deniers ask (compilation, German) and give the answer themselves: The scientists and «alarmists» are supported by massive amounts of government and private money, while they, the deniers, act selflessly, based solely on their (imagined) moral obligation to communicate what they consider to be the truth.

Before making a precipitated judgment, let’s try to put ourselves in the average active denier’s shoes. What, for example, drove this doubtlessly self-motivated young German to make a half hour video? He does not seek to propagate his name. I refuse to believe that he is seeking joy in deceiving the public —much the opposite. He seems even not to be closely associated with the various deniers in Germany who act more professionally. What is in for him? He hopes not to be accused for causing CO2 emissions, works towards that goal and discards own feelings of guilt. And he profiles as a nice guy who actively removes guilt from his audience.

The young denier’s video gets particularly interesting near the end (jump to 28:49 minutes here). He is worried about social justice —hates to see families having to pay for the energy transition—, disapproves of the induction of «fear» by climate alarmists, suggests a solution (why would he want to do that if there is no problem?) and, best of all, he sympathizes with voluntarily renouncing to the consumption of products which had a detrimental effect on the environment upon production.

The zeal should make us think about the causes of non-professional but very active climate denial. The deniers feel challenged or even offended.

What if the young denier had not been flooded with guilt-inducing climate preaching and «hysteria» including in school, but with the message that there is an effective, efficient (low cost) and socially fair solution to the problem? What if he had been told that climate change, caused by the burning of fossil fuels, is nevertheless, not his fault? What if he had been told that he is not guilty of climate change, but that he should please make a video to join the effort to solve the problem?

Retributions in anger

Few want to communicate the problem there is with global warming in the first place (implicit accusation inhibition). To reject the implicit accusation that underlies the climate change message, many deny the facts altogether (accusation rejection bias). Instead, they accuse the messenger of being part of a conspiracy or to be acting out of greed (accusation reflection effect).

Deniers try to decry the science on climate change by asserting that scientists exaggerate global warming because they want more funding. Politicians, they claim, make up climate change because they want more tax. As implausible and ridiculous as these assertions are, they are widespread.

If professional deniers assert it, it is easy to make an educated guess about the psychology at work: They project their own readiness to be corrupted onto others.

The motivations and the psychology of the many non-professional active deniers are more interesting than the motivations of the professional deniers, however.

The abundance of recriminations against climate scientists and the degree of anger that often goes with it is one of many indicators that, consciously or subconsciously, though inadvertently, the climate communicator’s message is perceived as an accusation.

The zeal, discussed in the previous section, and the anger of some deniers hints at their feeling hard-pressed to address the public and confront climate communicators. However, if these deniers are driven by an urge for defense and retaliation, what was the offense? The offense is the assertion that climate change is their fault.

The recriminations themselves made by climate deniers are not dealt with further in this article —but there are stories to be told by many climate scientists like Micheal Mann, stories that are reminiscent of the recriminations there were against Ignaz Semmelweis and his findings.

The reaction to Semmelweis‘ message and the reaction to climate communicators‘ message are closely related. However, there are significant differences between the two cases.

Semmelweis was very much a lone voice. Consequently recriminations and even outright retribution were directed at him, almost exclusively.

In contrast, climate preaching is widespread and done by most environmentalists. Consequently, the retributions as a reaction to climate preaching are rather unfocused. To some degree, the unwanted reactions negatively affect all climate communicators and even all environmentalists —although they fail to identify the exact cause for the reaction, climate preaching.

Another significant difference is that Semmelweis had little choice but to implicitly accuse his colleagues. To a large extent, climate communicators and environmentalists would have that choice.


Honest climate communication almost inevitably induces a notion of guilt or shame. This article focuses on denial induced through appeals for voluntarily individual behavior change because, unlike some other elements of climate communication, these appeals could be avoided.


Part 3 — Climate Preaching as a Problem

Accusation or blame, guilt or shame

This article refers to accusation rather than blame, although the latter word would often be more appropriate. Similarly, it mentions guilt when the feeling could be called shame. Although there is a difference between guilt and shame, for simplicity, the distinction is not made in this article. 6

Climate preaching and guilt avoidance

Climate preaching, i.e. calls for voluntary personal climate mitigation action, calls for individual lifestyle changes, typically consumer appeals, include an implicit accusation and induce guilt, rejection and denial —as Ignaz Semmelweis‘ findings induced guilt, rejection and denial. The resulting resistance hampers the conversation about climate change —in much the same way as the communication of Semmelweis‘ important findings was affected.

Titelbild von Das Magazin No. 26 – 2018. Wie konsumiere ich verantwortungsvoll. Es ist verdammt kompliziert. (Paula Scheidt)
«How can I consume responsibly? It is pretty darn complicated.» To find the answer is irrelevant because it is the wrong question. | Cover of Das Magazin No. 26 – 2018 with a piece by Paula Scheidt about the trickiness of green consumption —and its limits.

There is considerable evidence to support the hypothesis that appeals for voluntary personal acts to mitigate climate change cause denial because these appeals implicitly accuse and induce guilt and the avoidance of it. Different branches of psychology frame what might be the same problem in different ways.

Social norms and polarization

One known psychological problem with personal level moral climate appeals is that they, quite unwillingly but effectively, postulate a social norm. The appeal: «We should not do this» contains the message that we are actually doing it. To emit CO2 is a normal thing to do and people prefer to adhere to social norms, even if the continuation of a social standard may be collectively destructive.

However, those who already engage in voluntary acts or sympathize with such action appreciate the preaching, are reinforced in their view by the appeals that aim at establishing a new norm.

But the others, the unconvinced, reinforce their adherence to the existing social standard and reject the appeals, including, I dare to posit, by denying the problem.

Climate preaching, I also dare to posit, leads to a polarization. Conflicting norms induce a polarization on environmental action. In one segment of the population, it even induces indiscriminate rejection of environmental action.

The polarization effect due to conflicting norms was described by Rachel McDonald and colleagues (McDonald et al. 2014). Unfortunately, while the calls for changing their behavior positively affected the already convinced, the effect on what should be the target audience was negative, as is explained in this TED-talk by Winnifred Louis.

True, there is no open accusation in climate preachings, at least not in most of the many instances. But climate preaching always contains an implicit accusation and we should expect it to elicit rejection and denial, at least in one part of the population.

Overt expression of guilt avoidance

Some openly admit they reject(ed) the science on climate change to fend off the underlying accusation, as did one convert (Yale Climate Connections‘ Karin Kirk reported):

«I believed the ‘climate change is happening but humans aren’t the main cause’ bull. No idea why I thought it, guess it was just said enough and sounded good [because] it removed any blame from us (as a species).»

Denial didn’t just remove the blame from «the species». It removed it right from the quoted former denier.

Even though —for obvious reasons— no denier should have any reason to openly write about his avoiding guilt or shame, such statements can often be found, particularly in online-comments.

Denier’s blogs or videos often quickly reveal rather clear statements that indicate that climate messages are perceived as accusations and contribute to the denier’s position. One active denier devotes a whole blog post to it and writes: «We are being accused of being guilty to cause an event, that should happen in the future, catastrophic climate change.» («Man klagt uns an, wir wären schuldig. An einem Ereignis, das erst in der Zukunft stattfinden soll. An der Klimakatastrophe.»)

On the same site, I could find within minutes someone who made the link to climate preaching. He wrote in a comment (no. 15) criticizing climate scientists like Stefan Rahmstorf: «And what do you personally contribute to protecting the climate by personally limiting your consumption[?]» («Und was tragt Ihr persönlich zum Klimaschutz an Verzicht bei[?]») A climate friendly lifestyle is not the best option for a climate scientist to counteract the warming of the planet or counteract climate denial. He should not have to do it (and I think he should not do it), but Stephan Rahmstorf is actually not reluctant to talk about his low-carbon lifestyle —perhaps because he would be even more of a target for the deniers if he didn’t. (Rahmstorf’s ecological lifestyle is a topic at the beginning of a compilation of TV broadcasts which questions the science on climate change; Stefan Rahmstorf wrote extensively about his low carbon lifestyle on his blog; both in German.)

If climate scientists and communicators point out their eco-friendly lifestyle they may enhance their credibility. But they frame the problem as a personal moral challenge rather than a collective, a political challenge.

Climate communicators are aware that the framing they use in their communication is important and seek to optimize it for it to be appealing to the audience with the greatest affinity —often with the greatest affinity to climate preaching. However, I think they rarely consider how other people, how a different audience is likely to frame the same pieces of communication.

In the following sections, different ways to frame or interpret the problem of moral climate appeals are discussed.

Negative response to preaching as resistance to manipulation

Readers who want to see climate preaching as positive and beneficial should consider the arguments presented in this section with an open mind.

To follow a typical appeal for voluntary individual action is not beneficial for the recipients of the message. There may be an indirect, psychological benefit. As already pointed out, small positive reactions to the appeals serve as justifications for free riding. But, certainly, compared to no reaction at all, it is not normally otherwise beneficial to comply with the preaching. A positive response to appeals helps the rest of the community, including the preacher, but it disadvantages the positive respondent who renounces to certain things without direct gain to him.

Climate preaching may be and probably rather should therefore be interpreted as manipulation. Consequently, resistance to climate preaching may be framed as resistance to manipulation.

Climate preachers often try hard to make their audience believe that to follow their appeals is materially and directly beneficial for their audience —and probably the preachers often believe it, too, perhaps to mask from themselves that they are manipulators.

Unsurprisingly, the preachers and those who appreciate the preaching want to see it differently —often they are the same people as is explained in a subsequent section (the self enhancing cycle). Some of them probably think they will save the planet with what they consider respectable acts of altruism, indeed.

At the same time as they reckon their acts as reputable, if not extraordinary, some probably really believe that their example will be followed quickly, universally and that a new behavioral societal standard will be established and followed thoroughly, in the same way as in some societies religious credence may be followed with remarkable zeal, sometimes as if it were law.

If a new moral standard were attained and universally respected, the appeals would end up being beneficial societally, i.e. including for those who follow the appeals, indeed. Such a path and achievement would be possible if the appeals served to solve a problem in a small community or if other factors favored social control. Or it could be successful under very favorable other circumstances, e.g. if an innate human disposition came to help. But unfortunately, there is no instinct to counteract industrial pollution. A favorable development towards societal scale voluntary climate action as a consequence of climate preaching is too far off to hope for in the case of CO2 emissions from fossil fuels.

To «frame» reluctance or resistance to respond positively to climate preaching as negative behavior is far too simple. To resist manipulation is an extremely important human capacity and it is an underlying factor for several important psychological biases.

However, whether you, dear reader, see climate preaching as manipulation (or not) is irrelevant for the argument of this section. What counts is this: Do the deniers see climate preaching as manipulation and do they react to it? They do.

Put somewhat nonchalantly, a reaction —or reactance— to appeals could be described like this: «The climate preachers (and their followers) want me to go back to living in a cave, but this time without fur, without meat anyway, without even leather, but without solving a problem! I am not in for that!»

Via resistance to be manipulated, the preaching is a root cause for the somewhat diffuse but widespread aversion against anything ecological, against everything proposed by anybody identified as one of those with a pro-environmental mentality. Without the preaching, there would be far less reason for this kind of reactance.

Eco-preaching and our healthy capacity to identify and to reject manipulation —and to despise manipulators— plays a big part in the debate on climate change.

Ecological preaching and resistance to manipulation combine in a predictable and effective way with loss aversion. It results in a rather general aversion against everything and everybody «ecological». Combine it with groupthink, let confirmation bias do its job and the political polarization on climate change can be explained.

If our capacity to resist manipulation is given due merit, the aversion —particularly among deniers— against climate preachers (seen as manipulators or deceivers) becomes rather comprehensible.

The inconsistency problem with climate preaching

Assume somebody asked you to voluntarily change to a vegetarian diet, but the proponent of vegetarianism is admittedly (or reportedly) not a vegetarian himself. Wouldn’t you see the meat-eating proponent of a meatless diet as a manipulator or even as an outright deceiver? You would, for good reason. But consider the following.

In a modern society, it is impossible to live a lifestyle that is fully compatible with the requirements to mitigate climate change, i.e. zero carbon emissions. Therefore, nobody can ask anybody else to live a climate compatible lifestyle, without being seen very critically —as a manipulator— for good reason. In this offensive video (German), with deplorable style but some justification, a denier accuses Al Gore of preaching but not walking his talk. Gore is quoted and exposed for (incorrectly) claiming that a zero carbon lifestyle is possible as a personal endeavor.

Because climate preaching is so abundant, it negatively impacts on everybody who wants to communicate the CO2-challenge, even those who don’t support the moral climate appeals. For one example, there is a blog post (German) by a denier who accuses climate scientist Hans Joachim Schellnhuber of living a high carbon lifestyle by referring to an interview with the scientist. The denier accuses Schellnhuber even though the scientist makes a clear statement against climate preaching in the very same interview. (Please notice how topsy-turvy it got: The denier is a climate preacher and the non-denier, Schellnhuber, on the occasion argues against preaching. On other occasions, the scientist is also among the preachers, though, as is explained in this article; German.)

Moral climate appeals impact negatively on any climate communicators‘ message.

Many advocates of climate preaching claim that environmentalists should voluntarily reduce their carbon footprint because the environmentalists otherwise provide skeptics and contrarians with ammunition for criticism. (Sometimes self-centered environmentalists seem to even enjoy that politically oriented environmentalists are criticized unless they lead voluntarily by example. It is a strange kind of enjoyment, indeed!)

The advocates of preaching should realize, that the contrarians will never stop this kind of criticism, regardless of how much any environmentalist will reduce his carbon footprint. In fact, climate preachers provide ammunition to the skeptics by supporting their argumentation. Worse, these advocates assist the contrarians in their assault against politically focused environmentalists, directly —as well as indirectly by motivating the denial. However, this seems to be very difficult for climate preachers to understand.

The paradox of dire messaging and inadequate proposals

To launch dire messages but propose a completely inappropriate response is not suitable to increase trust in the climate communicators‘ message, either. Yet exactly this is done over and over again by climate preachers ringing the alarm. For one example, see the video below.

«12 years to save the planet from catastrophic climate change … What more will you do to save the planet?» Those who see themselves as world leaders, who meet annually in Davos, send an alarming message to the minor world citizens («Once the world goes beyond [1.5 degrees of warming], much or our planet won’t be fit for humans»). To conclude, the «world leaders» suggest to separate trash for recycling. | WEF Video from October 2018.

It is not very difficult to imagine how people skeptical of the climate message frame the behavior of climate preachers who send out alarming or even alarmist messages but propose an inappropriate solution: voluntary individual acts on the consumer level. This inconsistency of their messaging suggests to interpret climate preaching as manipulation and to see the climate preachers as manipulators.

«The end of civilization is near!» (There is little reason to believe this, but the prospect for many non-human living creatures is dire.) «We are about to hit a wall!» (Perhaps, but it might be an awfully soft wall, at least for Homo sapiens.) «We are running out of resources!» (When quite obviously there is more than plenty, again, as far as humans are concerned.) «Peak oil, peak gas, peak coal!» (When the CO2-problem quite obviously is due to the abundance and oversupply of fossil energy, the opposite of scarcity.) «Only so many years left to solve the problem!» (How many years will it be next time that this claim is made, but what about the Puerto Ricans it was too late for in 2017? Furthermore, how many times has the assertion been used as a pretense for complacency, a reason to let those years pass before action is taken?) «Climate is changing faster than ever!» (Although there are several ‹years without summer› in reliably recorded history; although lesser volcano eruptions regularly leave noticeable traces in global temperature measurements.) Finally, my personal favorite: «We are using so many planets!» (A claim made by just about every environmental NGO, despite its blatant lack of plausibility. The result of a calculation set up to produce a wanted result and mislead the public, but an understatement of the problem of fossil fuels and accumulating CO2.) Many environmentalists make such claims with great ease. Many more embrace them uncritically, including even the outrageous claim that climate change will soon kill nearly all of us.

The average environmentalist, I observe, is not even remotely aware of how skewed and potentially troublesome many of these familiar assertions are and that they are incompatible with climate preaching.

Framing of positive response to preaching

A possible framing of the desire to positively respond to climate preaching should also be stated.

To be aware of a severe problem and respond to it with acts that —reasonably judged— primarily serve to relief one’s bad conscience is an egocentric reaction to the problem. If the reaction also serves to be well seen by peers, it is clearly an egoistic response, although it may look and may be presented as the opposite.

Those who see environmental action critically, or oppose it outright, consider environmentalists as hypocrites. They do so for rather good reason, because the overwhelming majority of environmentalists focus on self-centered action. This article (German) points it out —but, nevertheless, as usual, embraces climate preaching.

Avoidance of cognitive dissonance

Social psychologists may prefer another framing and consider denial due to climate preaching as a case of avoiding cognitive dissonance.

One study by Susanne Stoll-Kleemann and colleagues working with focus groups in Switzerland looked at denial as a consequence of avoiding cognitive dissonance and made this observation (Stoll-Kleemann et al. 2001):

One area of consistency analysed here lies in the possible disjunction between a personal preference for a particular lifestyle, consumption habit, or behavioral choice and the need to respond effectively to climate change mitigation strategies. In short, people may profess anxiety over climate change, but be faced with internal resentment or even denial over what they cannot accept as a justifyable change in behaviour (e.g. to travel by public transport, ride a bike in the rain or invest in high cost domestic insulation). The research […] suggests nine ways in which this denial may occur:

¤ … metaphor of displaced commitment — I protect the environment in other ways
¤ … to condemn the accuser — You have no right to challenge me
¤ … denial of responsibility — I am not the main cause of this problem
¤ … rejection of blame — I have done nothing so wrong as to be destructive
¤ … ignorance — I simply don’t know the consequences of my actions
¤ … powerlessness— I am only an infinitesimal being in the order of things
¤ … fabricated constraints — There are too many impediments
¤ … „After the flood“ — What is the future doing for me?
¤ … comfort — It is too difficult for me to change my behaviour

These statements are telling alone. Yet, to this list could be added:

¤ … denial of the evidence — It’s not a problem or not as big a problem as you claim

In many studies that explore the psychology of climate denial, denial is seen as the refusal to acknowledge a fact —the observation of the warming or the physical basis. Sometimes it is seen as a refusal to act. In that case, it is almost always seen as a refusal to respond with individual, voluntary acts, rarely as a refusal to act societally and pursue a collective objective or a specific policy goal. This is also the case in a meta-study on the psychological causes of denial (Stoknes 2014). However, Stoknes recognizes the implicit accusation, omnipresent in climate communications, as a problem and mentions guilt:

The affects and emotions that have become dominant in attitudes toward this issue [climate change] are a mixture of underlying unease, fear and guilt created by a climate message that constantly repeats that we should drive and fly less, eat less meat and generally not consume so much to avoid disaster. (Stoknes 2014)

Stoknes mentions science denial as one form of reaction to dissonance in his TED-talk (at 10:54 minutes before the end). Interestingly, the statement «changing my diet doesn’t amount to anything, if I am the only one to do it» is portrayed as a form of denial. (If denial is defined as a failure to acknowledge a fact, it is objectively not denial because the statement is —overwhelmingly— correct. The effect of one person alone changing his or her diet is in fact negligible.) In a written summary, Stoknes lists barriers for climate communication and action. Number 4 barrier is denial. Stoknes writes:

Denial. When we negate, ignore, or otherwise avoid acknowledging the unsettling facts about climate change, we find refuge from fear and guilt. By joining outspoken denialism and mockery, we can get back at those whom we feel criticize our lifestyles, think they know better, and try to tell us how to live. Denial is based in self-defense, not ignorance, intelligence, or lack of information.

This is a perfect analysis, I think. However, to overcome these barriers, Stoknes, among other things, clearly recommends personal little acts on the consumer side which trigger the self-defense he himself has identified as the cause of denial.

In another presentation, near the beginning, Stoknes mentions that people in Norway are among the least concerned about climate change and that people in the US, Australia and Great Britain (Norwegians not in the study) least acknowledge that global warming is anthropogenic. (He accidentally talks of «oil countries», at 5:27.) The inhabitants of these countries have much reason to feel guilty about climate change.

There are several studies that strongly support the hypotheses presented in this article, including Stoknes work. (Some studies are mentioned or described above or below and listed as references.)

However, to my knowledge, there is no study that specifically explores or tests whether calls for voluntary behavioral change cause or reinforce climate denial —through their implicit accusation. (If you, dear reader, are aware of any specific research, I would be interested to know.)

When presented with the hypothesis, a senior social psychologist, expert in the field of climate change, pointed out that there is little incentive to test it, because the result could be anticipated. To deny a problem (that climate change is real, man-made and important) as a consequence of climate preaching would be an trivial case of avoidance of cognitive dissonance.

While there might not be much research on the consequences of (the induction of) guilt on denial generally, and probably no research specifically regarding negative effects of climate preaching, there is research on how feeling guilty affects individual action or intentions to mitigate climate change.

The backfire effect of guilt induction

A variety of studies aimed at finding out whether inducing guilt motivates environmental action. Results are mixed and seem to depend on the type of environmental problem and the type of action. At least two studies conclude: Feeling guilty or, more precisely, the induction of guilt can discourage climate action.

Megan Bissing and colleagues report on a study they made: «Results showed that pro-environmental behavior […] was positively related to pride, and negatively related to guilt […].» (Bissing-Olsen et al. 2016)

 

The influence of anticipated pride or guilt. (Schneider et al. 2017)
If made feel proud about making environmentally beneficial choices, respondents were significantly more inclined towards making green choices (black dotted-dashed line) as compared to a reference (solid grey). However, respondents were significantly less inclined towards green choices when previously made feel guilty (dark grey dotted line). A: preference for a green product over one with other advantages; B: readiness to choose a lower or higher number of green extras at a cost; C: likelihood of “buying a green product in the next month”; D: intention to perform a series of sustainable actions over the next month. The effect of induced guilt (or pride) vs. the control group („reference“) is noticeable in all 4 cases, but did not reach statistical significance. (Schneider et al 2017, full text available)

In another study, Claudia Schneider and colleagues tested green intentions and found basically the same result. If guilt was induced for having made (imaginary) non-ecological choices, respondents were more likely to reject —not accept— another (also imaginary but different) set of hypothetical green choices. (Schneider et al. 2017; graphs above and below)

Although the observed trends did not reach statistical significance, these results should make us think. Could the respondents‘ refusal to (intend to) make greener choices when feeling guilty be a case of accusation rejection bias?

The results are qualitatively the same for all choices but one: If made feel guilty (or proud), respondents were more inclined to send cash.

Graph from Schneider et al. 2017. The influence of anticipated pride or guilt. E-donation.
Induction of a feeling of guilt (grey dotted line) enhances the readiness to donate, as does the induction of pride (black dotted-dashed), albeit both without statistical significance. (Source)

Now, at last, we may make an educated guess about why environmental NGOs make their supporters feel guilty by telling them about all the things they should do to save the environment —but these supporters don’t follow the advice. Instead, they donate. However, even if all were just about NGO income optimization, there might be a better way to communicate the climate issue than to induce guilt or pride, as will be reported next.

The effect of making climate change your problem vs. our problem

Nick Obradovich and Scott Guenther studied how the framing of climate change as a personal problem vs. the framing as a collective problem (society, transportation, etc.) affects green intentions. A rare occurrence, the scientist’s considered that climate preaching might be problematic. In their introduction they write:

Many climate messages appeal directly to the individual’s role in emission reductions. For example, a Sierra Club newsletter touts “Five Simple Things You Can Do About Global Warming This Year.” Subsequent newsletters ask “How Green is your Laundry?”, “How Green is your PC?”, and “How Green is Your Vacation?”. This style of messaging, aimed at evoking feelings of personal responsibility, is common in advocacy organizations’ climate outreach. However, eliciting behavioral change is tricky. Along with the practical linking of climate cause, effect, and ameliorative action, personal responsibility messages may produce other, less helpful responses. Guilt, denial, sadness, and cognitive dissonance are all associated with recognizing one’s own role in the climate problem. (Obradovich and Guenther 2016.The authors cite references omitted in this quote.)

Study subjects were given tasks to internalize the framing of climate change as a personal or collective challenge:

(Personal:) In what ways do you cause climate change? You personally produce climate-change-causing emissions in a variety of ways. You may drive your car, fly on airplanes, and/or use fossil-fuel energy for heating or cooling, as examples. In the space below, please write a short paragraph about the ways you as an individual produce climate-change-causing emissions. How commonly do you engage in these behaviors? This paragraph should take you approximately 3–4 minutes to complete.

(Collective: ) In what ways is climate change caused? Climate-change-causing emissions are collectively produced in a variety of ways. Transportation – in the form of cars and airplanes – and the use of fossil-fuel energy for heating or cooling are examples. In the space below, please write a short paragraph about the sources of climate-change-causing emissions. How common are these sources? This paragraph should take you approximately 3–4 minutes to complete.

Surprisingly —or not— Obradovich and Guenther found that the treatment of climate change as a collective problem resulted in higher readiness to donate, both with environmentalists as with the general public (2 tests + 1 follow up) as well as to change behavior («How likely are you to reduce your own climate-change-causing behaviors in the future?», general public, 1 test).

Portraying climate change as a personal problem didn't increase donations, as "collective" did. (Obradovich and Guenther 2016
Portraying climate change as a personal problem (your fault, your challenge) did not increase willingness to donate. Portrayal as a societal and physical („collective“) problem (greenhouse gas emissions, transportation, etc.) did. (Obradovich and Guenther 2016)

A first and easy expectation would certainly have been that the framing as a personal problem would have considerable guilt-inducing effect, which would increase the willingness to donate compared to the control group. The results contradict this simple expectation. In one out of four tests (donations, general public, chart above), the scientists also assessed how the treatments increased negative emotions. «Neither condition created differential feelings of guilt», the scientists report. Induced denial might have dampened that group’s willingness to donate (my speculation). If this (my) theory is correct, it may further be guessed that guilt is not displayed or felt but immediately overcome by denial. An essay writing task might have given the personal treatment group an opportunity to remind themselves of their personal action and thus to overcome negative feelings —if such feeling were not displaced by denial anyway (my speculation, still).

The framing as a collective problem significantly induced negative feelings compared to a control group and moderately compared to the group treated with the personal framing, the scientists report. Perhaps, (also my speculation) the treatment as a collective problem suggested a cooperative solution via a donation.

The studies also evaluated respondent’s belief in the existence of climate change: «Around 85 % reported believing that climate change was occurring and a slightly lower percentage reported believing that climate change was primarily anthropogenically driven.» It would be interesting to know whether the believers or non-believers were affected differently by the different treatment.

Without much speculation, the study result strongly suggests that the portrayal of climate change as a collective problem induces a desire to act and to act collectively —but the portrayal as a personal problem does not.

Repetition as a way to overcome guilt

There are several ways to overcome a feeling of guilt, other than to deny the problem or engage in more or less meaningful action. One way to cope with the undesired feeling is to repeat the wrongdoing —it would be a form of repetition compulsion. Modern psychologists would more likely analyze the behavior as a form of reactance to climate preaching. Paradoxically, if I feel guilty about flying, flying again is a way to overcome feeling guilty about it. Doing something is a way of asserting that it is okay to do it.

There is a perfekt example by Julia Hartley-Brewer, equally liked as despised (added 2019-08-15).

This is a small section with only a bit more than one paragraph. Potentially, this is a severe underrepresentation of the problem. Repetition to overcome a feeling of guilt may be a big factor in guilt avoidance on climate change and a significant consequence of climate preaching.

More reasons to feel guilty about climate change

Other pieces of climate communication than calls for voluntary action can induce guilt and potentially have a negative effect: Exaggerating the problem by portraying the effects of greenhouse gas emissions as excessively harmful is one such way, portraying it as especially unjust another (issue of climate justice). The idea of climate debt should be seen critically, too, as well as the idea of degrowth, particularly when it is promoted as an end in itself.

However, climate communicators are much in the same situation as was Ignaz Semmelweis: If their communication is honest, it almost inevitably suggests a notion of previous wrongdoing, i.e. guilt.

Humans have a phenomenal capacity to foresee and avoid own wrongdoing or evaluate past behavior. Ethics almost inevitably provides a ground to feel guilty about our personal contribution to the changing climate. But, first, it may reasonably be doubted that intrinsic ethics alone may cause denial. Second, it should be explained that a personal contribution to the warming climate is inevitable, unless there is political change, i.e. policy change. Guilt induction can and should be counteracted where possible. Climate preaching could and should be avoided.

More reasons to view climate preaching critically

This article focuses on climate denial as well as climate silence induced through calls to voluntarily change individual behavior because the appeals could be avoided. However, there are several more reasons to dismiss appeals for voluntary action on climate change and particularly on fossil energy use:

  • They are a deception or a self-deception because they would have to be complied with universally which won’t happen.
  • Calls to voluntarily change our behavior belittle the political challenge associated with global warming. Climate preaching is a kind of climate denial.
  • By giving self-centered voluntary action prime status, calls for voluntary personal behavior change are a distraction and may preclude more meaningful societal action. Guilt can be a strong motivator. If at all, it should be employed to motivate effective, societally relevant action. Any year has no more than 366 days and any day 24 hours. Any substantial amount of self-centered activities takes away time from societally relevant action.
  • Finally, the habitual climate appeals are a waste of both time and money on the side of the climate campaigns. It would be strategically wise and efficient to concentrate climate campaigning efforts on the production side of fossil energy and on policy to transform the energy economy as well as setting up policy driven financial or legal incentives to change our lifestyles.

Whether climate preaching is considered a deception (first point in above list) or even manipulation is a matter of taste, definition or perspective. Objectively, climate preaching is dishonest communication. If the preaching is well received by people who follow the appeals, it is a foundation for self-deception: It pleases the minds of these people and makes them feel good. It even helps those who feel good to deceive others, because they don’t significantly help to solve the problem with their mind-pleasing little acts. However, the pleasing of the mind can backfire.

This backfiring deserves a significant remark, a specific section. It’s a small section. But it can be a big problem for those affected. It could even be their ultimate problem.

Climate preaching engenders desperation

According to my experience, those who think that the climate crisis should be overcome by personal voluntary change least believe that the problem can be solved. Their desperation results from the discrepancy between their belief that voluntary action should do it and their objective observation that it won’t. Actually, if their belief is fixed and their perception unobstructed, their desperation is inevitable.

As a consequence, they may get desolate or depressed. I could mention friends, but don’t have to because there is a prominent example.

French environmentalist and filmmaker Yann Arthus-Bertrand calls himself a desolate environmentalist. He believes that he and you should solve the problem with what we buy and what we eat, voluntarily and that politicians won’t solve the problem.

For him, people are not egoists but should be expected to act morally correctly, selflessly. It is a wrong expectation. Unsurprisingly, it begets desolation.

Journalist (0:16): «You say that you have changed a bit the way you talk about the ecology. In plain, you say you were perhaps wrong to accuse or make your audience feel guilty and that this was a bit inefficient as a strategy.» Arthus-Bertrand intervenes: «I am a lost and desolate environmentalist … we are the biggest consumer of meat in the world, meat is important for me, animal suffering (3:15) … How could you be optimistic with an ongoing mass extinction (04:06) … 6th mass extinction, that’s the future of our children, the future of humans … because we live in this world based on economic growth …» Journalist: «based on egoism, perhaps?» Arthus-Bertrand, intervenes: «No! It’s the growth … we can’t count on politicians to solve the problem, you, me and all of us are going to solve this problem … it’s you, you, you its us, we are going to solve this problem.» | Interview with Yann Arthus-Bertrand. In another video, entirely in French, with the title ‚If everybody ate organic, there would be no Monsanto‘ («Si on mangeait tous bio, il n’y aurait pas de Monsanto»), Arthus-Bertrand makes essentially the same statements but indicates even more clearly his belief that politicians won’t but consumer should solve the problem.

It is not a problem, certainly not a big problem, if someone quietly lives a green lifestyle and refrains from purchasing particularly problematic products to ease the pressure on the environment. But to advertise it is a problem, more than one.

There are more negative effects of climate preaching because it also negatively impacts on political action. (Or there might be significant positive consequences of climate preaching which will be the topic of part 4.)

Criticism of self-centered action but not of climate preaching

There is some criticism of voluntary acts of green consumption but practically zero criticism of climate preaching even though the first is certainly not a big problem, but the second is.

Jennifer Jacquet received considerable attention with her book Is shame necessary? She wrote about the power of shaming the politically strong, as opposed to shaming the politically weak, the consumers.

Recent examples (in German) that view individual consumer action critically are:

  • Paula Scheidt wrote on six pages about the difficulties to try and be a green consumer and concludes that she will henceforth not only consider to be a responsible consumer, but also a politically responsible citizen.
  • In his book (Ökoroutine) Michael Kopatz also approaches from the side of voluntary action and points out that a complementary political framework would help those who are inclined to believe in individual action, too, and that policy should be strengthened, limits should be set.
  • Silvia Liebrich recently advocated «limits» and wrote in the same vein: «To rely on people voluntarily renouncing to consumption would be naive and careless».
  • One notch up, frontline critic of eco-hypocrisy and corporate greenwashing Kathrin Hartmann (recent book: Die grüne Lüge, The Green Lie) keeps writing about corporate greenwashing and how eco-aware individuals fall for it. She goes as far as to deplore personal greenwashing, too, albeit without using this term.
  • In a column, Nina Kunz wrote that our self-centered view on consumption distracts from the production side and denounces the trend of «minimalism» as just another «narcissistic lifestyle» —an exceptional act of directness.

These views by the media, or conveyed through the media, might mirror unease with an observed deficit in the domain of political climate action. More likely, they mirror an unease with the shortcomings of individual voluntary consumer action. Either way, again, there is not a big problem with voluntary individual climate action on the consumer level. True, such action alone doesn’t make a noteworthy positive difference. The problem comes in when self-centered action is advertised or when it is there to be used as advertisement.

Experts in the field of climate communications, who also study ways to overcome denial, often advocate but rarely criticize little personal acts to mitigate climate change. Practically, they never criticize climate preaching, though —as far as I can see.

These experts make other recommendations to counter denial. How some of these recommendations align with the arguments of this article is discussed in the next sections.

Inoculation against manipulation

It has recently been stressed that pre-emptive inoculation or «prebunking» is helpful to counter climate denial, particularly to overcome pre-existing misconceptions or an unfavorable ideological worldview. Inoculation means to get the deniers to understand that their belief is a consequence of misinformation. It is then explained in which way the misinformation is conveyed. The recommendation to use inoculation is supported by evidence.

Moreover, there is a logic to it. Many deniers believe that global warming messages are part of an organized deception, a conspiracy —by governments, NGOs, corporations, activists and scientists, by Jews and Freemasons (Switzerland), the CIA, bankers or any combination of it.

Climate deniers are susceptible to conspiracy theories. This is supported by science and almost a precondition for climate denial, at least if climate deniers realize that almost all governments and almost all climate scientists accept the existence of anthropogenic global warming.

Because deniers are generally inclined to believe in conspiracy theories, they can be expected to be susceptible to the message and realization that they were (are being) manipulated indeed, but by the other side than they thought. They are being manipulated by the fossil fuel industry with their professional merchants of doubt (Germany).

Unfortunately, to tell the deniers they are being manipulated works on both sides of the controversy and the contrarians have so far been more able to put it to use. As explained above, the deniers have at least one objective reason to spot a manipulation and develop their wrongly held belief to be victims of a conspiracy. This manipulation is climate preaching.

Could it be that we climate communicators give the deniers too much reason to have faith in the wrong side? I think so.

It was already pointed out in previous sections that to preach but not be able to walk the talk may rather reasonably be interpreted as manipulation and that to point out the severity of climate change but propose inadequate action through preaching is an inconsistency that provokes suspicion. Cause for suspicion should be avoided —unless we want to support climate denial.

It is a good strategy to inoculate against climate denial. To seed doubt and suspicion with climate preaching is not a good strategy.

Is preaching just useless?

It is not claimed that preaching is useless in every case or domain. It makes sense to assume that the effect of moral appeals depends on the degree of social control there is to impose the wanted change of behavior and that it helps if the wanted change conforms to established ethics. The current efforts to get people to shift away from eating meat —under heavy preaching and social pressure— is a remarkable example. However, the example also demonstrates that progress with moral appeals is difficult to achieve in modern societies, even if conditions are favorable. Even in countries in which the number of vegetarians and vegans is on the rise, the reduction of meat consumption is slow, even on a per capita basis —if there is any reduction at all.

Particularly if social control is weak —as is normally the case in any large anonymous society— or if the personal benefits of the prevailing behavior are strong, we should not expect appeals to accomplish much change. Or maybe this:

I have seen many climate activists and campaigners shift from strenuous political, sometimes confrontational activities or even direct action towards self-centered action. But, with the exception of the exceptional Greta Thunberg, I do not know of a single person who has clearly moved in the opposite direction. Perhaps the social environment of climate activists induces them to adopt their peer group’s mainstream opinion that self-centered action counts. Then they settle with just this approach. It is a much more comfortable, non-controversial option than political action. Or, as activists get more and more entrenched in a specific social compound, they, consciously or unconsciously, adjust to social pressure from their peers.

I have adopted strange tactics to personally deal with pressure towards voluntary individual acts. An example:

My neighbor: «You know, I also think it’s a good move [to no longer have a car] because of what you do [be a climate activist and campaigner].»

My answer: «One more word like this and I won’t sell you that damn car!»

The shortcomings of moral preaching in its stronghold

That moral preaching often fails to have a positive effect, even on an ingroup audience, particularly when social control is weak (sexual practices) or absent (video), is known from where it is most pervasive: religion. Preaching is nevertheless abundant, including outside religious institutions or sects.

A study showed that children who were born into a religious community behave (within their group) less morally than children raised by atheists. This begs an explanation. The following is purely speculation, but an interesting speculation in our context. We all mentally keep book of our moral and immoral acts in order to avoid excessive misbehavior and as a result suffer from retaliation against us by those around us. Could it be that the preaching raises the impression in the (religious) group members that the group is morally correct which makes group members believe they act morally correctly, despite the facts? According to this speculation, members of religious groups count their adherence to a supposedly moral group in their moral bookkeeping as if group adherence were a moral act in itself and they consequently compensate by acting less morally, factually. Again, this is speculation, but the possibility should be considered.

If that speculation is right, might it by analogy explain, in part, that those who identify as environmentalists act, on the personal level, less environmentally friendly than average —because they identify as environmentalists, members of a morally superior group, with morals being conveyed through environmental appeals far more than moral acts? The possibility should not be discarded.

A more obvious explanation, but a rather superficial explanation, is that environmentalists are wealthier than average and can more easily afford a wasteful lifestyle. But if that is the sole reason we must ask: Why are wealthier people more likely to identify as environmentalists, e.g. more likely to vote green? Are they, maybe, less environmentalist on the personal level because they are politically more environmentalist than average? That possibility should not be discarded either. But a more compelling explanation, one that is based on the reasonable assumption that rich people will almost inevitably spend more, including on environmentally harmful activities, is this: People are more likely politically oriented environmentalists when they live wasteful lives —i.e. they are politically oriented environmentalists because they live wasteful lives. This last possibility is worth serious consideration.

Preaching is often useless, yet it abounds

The UNFCCC is the key institution supposed to resolve the greatest tragedy of the commons of all times —which is to be done by restricting freedom in the global commons. As if it had to disguise its weakness, the UNFCCC calls for voluntary consumer action and modern indulgences —dubbed compensation.

The IPCC is the central scientific body on climate change. Unless it changes the text before publication, it will declare a need for a «large scale behavior and lifestyle change (very high confidence)» in its upcoming report on the 1.5° target. (Note in retrospect: It did change the text. In the final version of its Summary for Policymakers the IPCC states: «Strengthened multi–level governance, institutional capacity, policy instruments, technological innovation and transfer and mobilization of finance, and changes in human behaviour and lifestyles are enabling conditions that enhance the feasibility of mitigation and adaptation options for 1.5°C consistent systems transitions. (high confidence)»)

In his groundbreaking book Don’t even Think about it! George Marshall, a climate communications specialist and former campaigner with Greenpeace, writes about the psychology of climate change and its denial. One notable chapter is called How Climate Change Became your Fault. In it he describes how governments regularly tried and succeeded to shift blame and responsibility from politicians —i.e. themselves— to consumers.

Marshall writes how their campaigns to raise consumer awareness failed systematically or turned out to be counterproductive. «Worse still», Marshall writes on the direct effects of the appeals, «these campaigns did not actually work.»

«In Australia», Marshall reports, «people became even less climate clever and a third fewer people considered climate change to be their most important issue after the campaign than they had before.» He writes: «Behind their [government’s] uplifting slogans, and their appeals to national unity, what they were really saying was: „Climate change is your fault.“» And:

And here lies the problem. As soon as one creates responsibility, one creates blame. Blame creates resentment, and talk of responsibility in the home makes that resentment very personal indeed. What none of us fully appreciated at the time was how readily these anodyne messages would be mobilized to fuel people’s sectarian prejudices. (p. 195)

It is almost as if Marshall had been tasked with writing a summary of this article, in just one paragraph. Or in two concise paragraphs:

Somehow, this was all the wrong way around. Those campaigns urging people to take personal responsibility and work together to „save the planet“ were saying the wrong things to the right audience and the right things to the wrong audience. So much for the hope that small personal lifestyle changes might shift people’s attitudes and bring people together; if anything, they seem to reinforce peoples prejudices and drive them apart. (p. 195/196)

Marshall criticizes himself for having been a climate preacher, too:

Books proliferated telling people to Measure their Carbon Footprint, get Low-Carbon, become Eco-Friendly, Save the Earth for a Fiver, Tread Lightly on the Earth, Kick the Fossil Fuel Habit, go on a Climate Diet. Or go on a Carbon Detox—the title of my own contribution to this short-lived and rapidly remaindered eco-tastic subgenre.

«Remaindered», perhaps, but a «short-lived» subgenre, certainly not. Marshall goes on to write:

Maybe we all went too far and, in our eagerness to find homey messages that would engage people, we fell into the wicked trap of limiting climate change through the solutions we proposed. (p. 193)

Marshall concludes the chapter with a paragraph that could be a perfect conclusion of this article, with one important exception: He just cannot detach from the seemingly irresistible notion of moral correctness (which, by the way, is so wrongly called political correctness). The adherence to «voluntary measures» indicates that Marshall’s wish for individual moral behavior to play an important role is stronger than his observations and conclusions.

What is missing, and what is urgently required is a coherent policy framework that provides a contract for shared participation—whether through voluntary measures or, as many campaigners now demand, some form of tax, ration, or dividend—within which personal actions are recognized and rewarded alongside equally important contributions from government, business, and fossil fuel companies. Not the power of one, but the power of all. (p. 197)

«Voluntary measures» and «equally important contributions from governments, business, and fossil fuel companies» hint at an amazing incoherence to come. Indeed, in his very next chapter, Marshall turns around swiftly and completely —or rather falls in the same «wicked trap» again. He devotes his whole next chapter to blaming the least responsible for global warming, the individual consumer, including climate scientists who don’t abstain from flying! Marshall also again criticizes himself, this time for being a frequent flyer!

To refrain from preaching seems to be very difficult for climate communicators. It might be because the preaching is appreciated by those preached upon. Appeals are appealing. Why is that?

«You control climate change. Turn down, switch off, recycle, walk, change.» Strangely, those who could and should solve the climate crisis delegate its solution to those who cannot. Even stranger, those who cannot solve the problem don’t object. | Climate preaching by the European Commission.
Appeals are welcome —but why?

When governments or corporations preach —or NGOs—, they are diverting responsibility away from themselves. One should therefore expect the public to reject climate preaching overtly and strongly. It could be seen and denounced as an inappropriate or even embarrassing act, as a deception or even as outright manipulation. But, the opposite is the case. With the exception of some deniers and a few (very few) on the other side of the climate frontline, appeals are universally welcome. This begs an explanation.

To a certain extent, climate appeals might be a legacy of the oil price crises of the 1970s. When OPEC throttled the supply and there was shortage at the pump, shortage of even plastic bags, a soaring price of transportation fuels and consequently an economic crisis, many governments imposed rationing. Furthermore, scarcity and the market mechanism —higher prices— regulated demand. However, appeals to save energy also helped —at least a little bit— to prevent the worst. Japan resorted to appeals with very notable success to keep its economy going after the 2011 Fukushima disaster and the shutdown of its nuclear reactors.

However, these were acute crises. Climate change is not and (unfortunately) won’t ever be an acute crisis (in which case it would be addressed with urgency and fervor). Moreover, the overabundance and systematic oversupply of fossil fuels, which causes the CO2-problem, is the opposite of what was the temporary, acute and artificially provoked scarcity of fossil fuels in the 70s or the acute power shortage in Japan provoked by natural disaster and technology failure. Furthermore, there is a human instinct to address shortages and scarcity, including with appeals and social control, but there is no instinct to address pollution caused by abundance and oversupply.

«Everybody saves energy together.» Pamphlet of a state supported energy savings campaign 1981
«Everybody saves energy together.» It’s been hanging there for decades, doing nothing. Or was its overall effect negative? | Pamphlet of a state supported energy savings campaign following the acute oil crises of the 70s, Switzerland. Appeals were remarkably successful. But they failed as the acuteness waned. Today it is attempted to solve the climate crisis with appeals again, even though the persistent and systematic oversupply of fossil fuels is fundamentally a different problem than was the acute scarcity of fossil transportation fuels, artificially caused by OPEC. Climate change is very much the opposite problem of the past oil crises.

Common denominators of the oil crises of the 1970s and climate change are the words fossil and energy. Apart from that, they could hardly be more different issues. Governments are nevertheless seeking to address both problems with the same method: appeals for voluntary behavior change. They do this even though they could have learned from the past: As the acuteness of the oil crises waned, so did the effect of consumer appeals.

Without interruption, since the oil crises, people in much of the industrialized world are flooded with appeals to save energy. To a certain degree we might simply have become accustomed to it, consider it as normal or even desirable. Perhaps it simply never occurred to us that there are downsides to these appeals.

If I were Shell, Exxon or BP, I would also advocate personal voluntary carbon footprint reductions and welcome climate preaching by others, knowing that the incessant stream of appeals won’t ever change my business case. And I would keep pointing out that people just keep demanding fossil fuels, hoping that people won’t see me as guilty but see themselves as guilty and in charge as consumers.

Climate preaching legitimizes the business case and societal status of the fossil energy industry, while it undermines the legitimacy of policy-relevant climate campaigning. It is no surprise that appeals are appealing in the camp that despises policy change to mitigate global warming and profits from the notion that consumers are guilty.

But, strangely —unfortunately— climate preaching is appealing in the opposite camp, in the camp of the environmentalists, by at least the same measure. Obviously, there is an innate tendency not only to launch but also to welcome appeals. 7

There may be several more answers to the intriguing question of why appeals are welcome. An innate disposition, a legacy not so much from the oil crises but rather from the Paleolithic, would be my primary guess, but there may be other significant reasons.

To preach is to make a pledge, albeit implicitly. The preacher’s audience has an interest to hear the pledge and therefore welcomes the preaching.

Those who engage in individual action view themselves as exemplary benefactors. Consequently, they are pleased by the appeals that confirm their belief that their self-centered little acts are important.

Another big driver for the near-universal acceptance of climate preaching is that the advertisement of individual voluntary acts reassures everybody who seeks to escape feeling guilty and to escape being viewed as guilty by others. It is reassuring if they say what I do —or what I could easily do or what I pretend to do— is the right thing to do. Confirmation and desirability biases are at work, once again. If one sees oneself as a leader and praise comes from followers or even preachers, that is a welcome confirmation.

Steve Westlake interviewed «leaders» who limit their flying and may be seen as leaders by example. The full list of their reported «reasons to not fly/self-declared role of leadership» is telling:

  • ‚Be able to look myself in the mirror‘, make a personal sacrifice, pre-empt future culpability for climate impacts (in reference to child), didn’t like flying, cause/force others to consider impacts of flying. (Katie Mitchell, theater director)
  • Actions consistent with science/job, set example of low-carbon living, role model for social change, position solidified by other’s confrontational reactions, didn’t like flying. (Alice Larkin, professor of climate science and energy policy)
  • Avoid causing environmental damage, pledge to self, set example of self-regulation, avoid hypocrisy, avoid cognitive dissonance, highlight environmental cost of flying. (George Monbiot, author, journalist)
  • Actions consistent with science/job, avoid cognitive dissonance, challenge social norms, set example of low-carbon living, role model for social change. (Kevin Anderson, professor of energy and climate change)

To act in order not to feel guilt, to advertise ones acts in order not to be seen guilty or, ideally, be seen as a leader by example, goes hand in hand.

The self enhancing cycle

There is a self-reinforcing cycle. Let us start with appeals:

  • Step 1: Appeals for moral acts, in our case: climate preaching.
  • Step 2: A positive response, e.g. an act of green consumption.
  • Step 3: To confirm the validity of his act, to himself as well as to others, the positive respondent advertises it.

The advertisement primarily serves his reputation or to make a positive impression. With advantage, the impression management is done indirectly, by nonverbal communication, e.g. by driving a hybrid car, installing solar panels, wearing a shirt or otherwise indirectly as a leader by example. Or it is done even less directly, but more actively, by proposing the act itself to others —by preaching. The respondent, perhaps a new convert, so takes the role of a (climate) preacher and we are back in step 1, preaching.

This cycle was certainly essential for establishing informal rules in pre-civilization societies (see also note 7).

It can now be observed at work with appeals for not eating meat, the spread of vegetarianism and veganism. There is a joke going around: «How can you tell a vegan?»; «No need for you to tell because, sure, he will tell you!»

The cycle enhances the esteem and the self(-esteem) of those who participate in it. And the cycle enhances itself.

It is not restricted to the existing eco-lifestyle faction. It may be supported by the acquisition of new converts and new preachers. Alas, the cycle goes on and, whether it is desirable in a particular case or not (i.e. to address global warming), it may spread like a Ponzi scheme, a self-nourishing carousel.

Collection of titles with eco-appeals and climate preaching
There is an abundance of recommendations to solve, with an egocentric approach, what may be the most difficult problem in human existence. There is often a component of self-aggrandizement: «My Heart Beats Green!», «I Buy Nothing!», «We Are the Change», «My Path to Zero Waste», «And now WE will save the world!» and there is often a remarkably broad claim: «Simply change the world», «50 simple things to save the world and save money». | Covers of selected publications. German publications only.

In the case of environmental action and climate change, the immediate effect of the eco-lifestyle advertisement remains slim but the stream of calls for green consumption is inevitably perceived beyond the eco-lifestyle faction, with no small impact.

Should we care about the potentially negative effects of climate preaching and impression management?

It is easy not to worry about the cycle of climate preaching and impression management. I used to maintain a position of indifference about the cycle too. It does little good, but no harm, so I thought and: Let them be happy with it! However, this indifference is wrong because of the cycle’s terrible effects, above all climate denial and climate silence.

It is wrong to remain indifferent about the cycle of climate preaching and impression management through more climate preaching —and more induction of guilt.

This impression management should be called what it is: Personal greenwashing.

The trend is even going the wrong way, away from the advertisement of political action to the advertisement of individual action. Greenpeace, the one very important international environmental NGO, so far neither reluctant to address the politically powerful, nor shy to denounce corporate greenwashing, launches campaigns that address the least powerful and help them with their personal greenwashing.

Researchers, particularly social scientists, try to confirm the belief that individual voluntary acts on climate count —which, supposedly, often include the researcher’s own acts. In his dissertation, Steve Westlake tried to prove that not to fly and talk about it (leading by example) has a positive effect —and, not surprisingly, succeeded with it. One critical interviewee commented: «This whole survey seems designed to produce answers supportive of the thesis that leaders not flying is an important climate communications strategy.» I’d essentially agree with this interviewee and dare to predict that Westlake limits his own flying and sought confirmation for the effectiveness of his doing so. (It would be interesting to know if there is a pattern: Do scientists who study the effectiveness of climate preaching tend to seek to reassure themselves about their own environmental attitude?) It might not have been intended, but Westlake’s work produced interesting results in support of the arguments of this article (reported above and below).

Should scientists, NGOs and state sponsored campaigns really help individuals clear their bad conscience about climate change? And should they help these individuals to self-aggrandize? Should individual consumers be assisted with their self-centered acts and their personal greenwashing? I don’t think so.

If it is posited and accepted that voluntary acts and personal greenwashing won’t solve the climate crisis; if it is accepted that societal-level action counts («political» action, if you prefer); if we want to know whether climate preaching is justified or should be rejected, then we must investigate further the interaction of climate preaching with society and the societal response. Some negative components of this interaction will be the topic of the following sections.

Climate communicators, we need to talk!

Out there are probably millions of calls to go vegan or refrain from air travel, but there are few calls to get organized globally or otherwise address climate change properly.

Yet, so far, there is neither any critical research nor an educated debate about climate preaching and its negative effects.

Almost entirely missing is an educated debate about who is really at fault. Who really is responsible for global warming? Genevieve Guenther approached this important topic. She wrote: «The we [are] responsible for climate change is a fictional construct, one that’s distorting and dangerous. By hiding who’s really responsible for our current, terrifying predicament, we provides political cover for the people who are happy to let hundreds of millions of other people die for their own profit and pleasure.» How true.

However, the potential of detaching from the notion of «we» is still not exploited and the wrong ones are still held accountable. The fact that climate change is not the consumers‘ fault fails to be acknowledged in Genevieve Guenther’s piece as well: «But constrained choices are not akin to the unthinking complicity of the 10 percent who produce 50 percent of global emissions every year by taking multiple long-haul flights for pleasure travel, heating their homes instead of putting on a sweater, and driving swollen SUVs that they replace every few years.»

«Wir bestimmen, was gekauft wird! Also am Ende bestimmen wir ja die Regeln.» («We determine what is purchased! Well, in the end, we are determining the rules.») Moritz Neumeier, with link to video.
«We determine what is purchased! Therefore, in the end, we determine the rules.» He asks whose fault it is, but gets the answer all wrong. Ecological consumer choices are rather the opposite of making rules. | Click on picture to start video by Moritz Neumeier.

The question of who is guilty of or responsible for climate change is rarely even asked. If so, it is almost always wrongly answered. Could it be that our appreciation of the problem and our allocation of responsibility is terribly distorted by the ubiquitous climate preaching? It could. That is not a far-fetched assumption, I think.

Artist and video blogger Moritz Neumeier advocates consumer action in place of political action. He is aware that consumer action always fails (video, German) and even ridicules his own failure to act somewhat consistently as a consumer. What if he understood that not the consumer, but he is guilty, because he preaches and accuses the wrong people?

Guilty is not the consumer. He is least guilty because he is the weakest and the most dispersed actor in this global tragedy of the commons. But he gets nearly all the blame. Thank you, climate preachers.

Prevalence of and preference for climate preaching in the media

Following the publication of the IPCC’s special report on 1.5 C warming, CNN produced a list of «what consumers can do» and asked WWF’s lead climate scientist Chris Weber «whether consumers can help meet this goal [of staying within 1.5 C of warming]», Weber responded: „Unequivocally, yes.“

There is no doubt that consumers could do something. The only question is whether they will. True, if everybody quit using fossil energy then CO2-emissions dropped to a level bearable for a long time (until also CO2 from cement production needs to be eliminated). The ubiquitous claim: «if everybody … » is nevertheless just wishful thinking. The big problem with the statement is the little word if.

Also as a consequence of the IPCC’s special report, Vox‘ Gaby del Valle interviewed Richard Heede, the co-founder and co-director of the Climate Accountability Institute, who said: «You can measure a person’s impact, but there would be a lot of digits behind the zero in terms of percent of global emissions attributable to or savable by an individual. We’re talking about 7 billion people on the planet.»

That is a rational assessment. Furthermore, Heede was quoted like this: «Individual consumers can’t be blamed for our rising global temperatures — but people want to feel like they’re doing something, no matter how small, to prevent the worst-case climate catastrophe scenario from unfolding.» And: «There is some personal satisfaction in doing right by ourselves as well as our grandchildren. We can’t solve the problem by ourselves, but it would be a morally better choice to attempt to do something and derive satisfaction by it, rather than saying, “My carbon savings don’t matter.” Because they do matter! They matter symbolically. They matter financially. They matter morally. They matter to your neighbors.»

They matter and don’t matter at the same time? From the media report, it is not sure what Richard Heede’s position really is. There is reason to believe that he sees the potential of voluntary individual acts rather critically (unless theses acts pay out financially). He concludes his bio (from 2012): «[Hede’s] passive-solar rammed-earth home cuts carbon emissions by two-thirds. To offset these savings he flies a 1958 Cessna 182.» However, the interview, «condensed and edited for clarity», is hardly perceived by most readers as skeptical of the voluntary consumer approach to climate change.

There is little but at least some skepticism about green consumption. However, so far, there is next to no criticism of climate preaching. This is remarkable because there is not much wrong with little personal acts, although ineffective they may be. But it is plain wrong to claim they are effective. It is wrong to preach.

The little bit of criticism there is of climate preaching comes from experts who understand that political action counts and that individual action might stand in its way. But, the few times when experts criticize climate preaching, it is routinely softened or inverted by the media.

The lack of orientation or even outright reversal on the matter in the media is salient. It is (for example) demonstrated in a Spiegel article by Lena Puttfarcken. Consulted experts realize and state that our self-centered little eco-acts fail to do the job and that they might just serve to avoid feeling guilty. So, what does the journalist recommend? Keep doing it anyway! Keep calculating your carbon footprints and let us get better at our personal greenwashing, our beloved Ersatzhandlungen (displacement activities)!

Michael Kopatz wrote a book about his conviction that (green) consumption follows (green) policy or, more specifically, that policy must set limits to consumption, which will turn everybody into a better consumer. In an interview, his opinion was flipped 180 degrees by a media —or journalist— fixated on climate preaching, Kopatz reports.

Experts who are critical of the self-centered consumer approach rarely say it. If they do, the media, fixated on individual action, often distort their view.

Some preach, others follow the preaching or pretend to do so. Most people remain seemingly unaffected but too silent. The climate is changing but not much seems to be changing on climate change, not on the societal level. There is one exception, though: The deniers go stronger than ever.

«Thank you!» think the deniers

The professional merchants of doubt, the professional «disinformers» (Joe Romm), profit from the cycle of preaching, self-centered acts and more preaching. Their many misled followers are guilt-driven. Particularly the followers and parrots within the system of climate denial are also little consumers.

But, unlike the eco-lifestyle faction, they refuse to pick up the blame. They reject the implicit accusation that is inevitably conveyed through climate preaching. To avoid feeling guilty—or to avoid cognitive dissonance—, they employ their own psychological mechanism and deny the problem. Driven by guilt-avoidance and confirmation bias, supported by the deniers‘ disinformation bubble of alternative climate facts, they become active disinformers, too. An army of non-professional climate disinformers floods the comments sections and social media content of the internet, thereby mutually reinforcing one another and creating ever more content for ever more deniers to source from.

Some of the more ambitious or successful of the non-professional deniers then receive funding and support from the fossil fuel industry via professional deniers and become themselves professional deniers.

The fossil fuel industry and the professional deniers are the primary profiteers from climate preaching. Certainly, they appreciate the preaching. Unfortunately, we in the opposite camp keep appreciating it, too, and we keep on preaching, regardless of the damage it causes, regardless of whether we play straight into the hands of the most repugnant of our adversaries. Why do environmentalists do that?

One cause may be simple: Environmentalists‘ repugnance of excessive consumption —levels of consumption that drive many environmentalists nearly crazy—, may be driving us to excessively appreciate consumers who don’t consume excessively. And it may be driving us to excessively appreciate climate preaching.

Moral climate appeals as a reaction to conspicuous consumption

For many, shopping has become a preferred spare time activity. As a reaction, many environmentalists are repelled by the rampant consumption. They are disgusted by people shopping as a hobby, shopping out of boredom, shopping stuff they don’t know whether they need it, shopping stuff they even know they don’t need.

Shopping while they drawn. | Tourist with Louis Vuitton bag wading through flooded Venice.
Patterns of consumption are seen as a problem. Not so with political apathy. Even less so with climate preaching. | Luxury shopping in flooded Venice, 2018.

Many of those who believe in a solution of climate change by voluntary consumer action see oversupply, overconsumption and the widespread addiction to shopping as a problem —perhaps even more than they see climate change as a problem.

It is comprehensible that consumption and consumers are despised and seen at fault. To conclude from there that consumers should also solve the problem of environmental degradation seems to make sense.

As much as this conclusion is comprehensible, it is wrong because, even if the consumer is at fault, he is the least capable of solving the problem.

If the global problem of fossil fuels could be resolved by means of voluntary personal acts, informal rules and social control, all these measures should strongly and universally be welcomed and appealed for.

Unfortunately, climate change cannot be resolved by the many billions we are, with virtually no exception, all of us changing our behavior thoroughly, voluntarily. Certainly, this is not going to happen in due time.

Action legitimizes preaching (another reason why climate preaching is welcome)

If limits to our shopping and consumption are embraced, it makes sense to embrace climate preaching, too, which is why the two usually go together indiscriminately. If, however, one of the two, consumer action on climate change or climate preaching, is seen critically, it is useful to keep the two, the voluntary act itself and the advertisement of it, apart. But it is rarely done. Why are the preaching and the act itself psychologically (and practically) difficult to detach from one another?

I propose an answer and also probe into the more important and more intriguing question why climate preaching is hardly ever seen critically. (If the problems with consumer appeals are recognized at all, climate preaching is probably never seen nearly as critically as in this article.)

It was already mentioned that, basically, individual voluntary consumer action is not a big problem, if it were done privately and quietly without using it for impression management. «Privately» and «quietly» practically never happens in this domain, though.

But we are free to imagine that it could happen privately and quietly. In that case individual voluntary consumer action would do no good to a degree that it would be worth mentioning. But it would do no harm either, we could conclude. At least in this rather hypothetical case, there seems to be no problem. Yet, there are problems.

In a way, one problem is threefold manifest in the words individual, voluntary and consumer:

  1. Like virtually all other environmental problems, the problem with fossil fuels is much better solved on the production side than the side of consumption, both for practical and for psychological reasons.
  2. The problem with CO2 should be recognized as a collective problem, not as an individual problem because it must be solved collectively.
  3. Most importantly, it must be solved with agreements and laws, not voluntariness.

Notwithstanding the importance of these issues, they are still no reason to condemn someone who discretely renounces plane travel, for example.

Many psychologists working on climate change usually try to alter people’s voluntary personal behavior on climate change and they wrongly (wrongly!) interpret people’s refusal to reduce their carbon footprint as a form of climate denial. The opposite, i.e. a positive response to climate preaching, is apprehended as an indirect acknowledgement of manmade global warming.

However, as is already hinted at above (e.g. in the section A strange pattern of preaching and denial) this view is too simple. Personal voluntary carbon footprint reductions are rather a form of denial of anthropogenic climate change than an acknowledgement of it. As is also explained in yet another previous section, self-centered voluntary action and denial are both due to the same psychological disposition, the avoidance of feeling guilty; both reactions aim at not being seen as guilty; both serve the same greater aim, the dodging of risk and work towards effective societal action on climate change; both are a response to climate preaching.

Those who respond to climate change with personal voluntary action acknowledge to themselves, and transmit with their response, their belief that this response as adequate, which it is not.

  • Because climate communications specialists, including psychologists, don’t recognize that individual voluntary consumer action is a form of denial, they (so far) don’t see it critically at all. Consequently, they have no reason to evaluate climate preaching separately from personal carbon footprint reductions and also have no reason to see climate preaching critically.

One scientist, expert in the field of climate communications, sees individual consumer action as only mildly positive and would rather like to see more societally relevant action happen. Yet, she does not at all condemn climate preaching —although climate preaching handicaps political action, I shall soon claim. Perhaps, it is simply too difficult to accept fully that climate preaching causes denial, when consumer action, i.e. the aim of the preaching, is interpreted —misinterpreted— as the opposite of denial.

Yet, there may be simpler ways to explain why practically everybody, including experts, fail to acknowledge the problems there are with climate preaching.

  • Practically everybody has a reason to avoid guilt on climate change. This makes superficial moral acts and moral appeals an attractive proposition.
  • The appeals have become so widespread that almost everybody adheres to them. Nearly all climate experts appeal actively for personal carbon footprint reductions themselves. Strangely, even experts who explicitly acknowledge climate preaching as a cause of denial do it. (For example Per Espen Stoknes or George Marshall, as is explained in two different sections above.)

Don’t get me wrong. Individuals emitting less CO2, without more technology, without more investment, without more material, would be great. Although it can only contribute moderately at best, it would be the most cost effective and most environmentally friendly contribution. Therefore, it is difficult to recognize the problem.

The problem is not the consumer action itself —or renouncing consumption. It is the proposal or suggestion that people should or will do it voluntarily; i.e. the problem is climate preaching in all its many forms. But that is difficult to acknowledge if the voluntary action and the preaching are appreciated en bloc.

To do consumer action justice, let us look at its potential and how it could and should be deployed.

Making the consumers act

It may be claimed that broad appeals for consumer action might lead up to powerful and effective boycotts. If so, great! However, proof for that has yet to be delivered. So far, calls for consumer boycott show little to no effect. It would be foolish to count on it.

Palm oil is a candidate case for significant consumer boycott. The destruction of tropical rainforests is an extremely pressing problem and the expansion of palm oil plantations is a big contributor. To hope for a big enough consumer strike to make a difference, and to work towards it, is tempting because it is not self-evident to identify how the problem could otherwise be addressed from a distance. (There are Rang-Tans in our bedrooms. But we don’t know what to do; video below, article.) I sincerely hope to be proven wrong with my guess that consumer action won’t even contribute much to solving this problem.

Be aware, even if palm oil production ceased to increase due a reduction of demand, tropical rainforests won’t be safe. There are many more business cases for destroying them (logging, mining, charcoal, industrial or small farming). They won’t be protected by consumer action, but by laws enacted by responsible politicians elected or motivated by citizens who understand what responsibility means. Or the rainforests will be destroyed.

It is absurd and rather against the claim made above that moral acts and its advertisement are nearly always welcome: When a company made a significant move towards boycott, to advertise it was banned out of concern «it doesn’t comply with the political rules». | Banned advertisement video.

The temporary boycott of Shell due to Greenpeace’s action on Brent Spar in 1995 was a different case and a remarkable success. Shell, however, was not boycotted for selling gasoline and gasoline’s detrimental environmental effects. If well done, to target one company with a boycott might be possible. But to target the entire fossil fuel industry would be quite a different challenge.

Consumer action incentivized by carbon pricing is yet another approach. It is policy-driven, very effective and therefore all different from a response to climate preaching. Unlike climate preaching, carbon pricing would (also) deliver significantly on the consumption side of the equation. This is one important reason why carbon pricing is highly efficient, i.e. cost-effective.

Consumer action and simply not consume is not a bad thing. It is a good thing. Let’s make it happen! Carbon pricing would make it happen.

The alternative is command and control. If I am not allowed to drive a car that uses gas or diesel, I won’t do it. If it is too expensive, I won’t do it either.

Paradoxically, those who claim that consumers should and will do the right thing without a proper legal or monetary incentive are an obstacle to consumer action because their opinion is an obstacle to the implementation of policy that would make consumers act, indeed.

It has become rather clear that humankind can solve the climate crisis without renouncing any important achievement provided by technology, energy use and industrialization.

If, hypothetically, the climate crisis could not be solved without giving up an important part of these achievements, however, we would have to be ready to give some of them up. Imagine, as a purely theoretical consideration, that there were no other energy source than fossil fuels and no way to sequester CO2 from flue gases or from the atmosphere and store it in the earth crust or convert it to methanol or other high-energy fuels. In that case, a readiness to change our lifestyles severely would have to be advertised and even demanded.

However, even in this purely theoretical scenario, an agreement to act collectively would have to be demanded, not unilateral voluntary lifestyle changes. Even in this hypothetical scenario, there would be no reason, no need and no justification for climate preaching. It would have to be demanded that every individual lives a simpler lifestyle if this solves a problem that must be solved. The CO2 problem must be solved. The policy necessary to achieve this has to be enacted. Policy change has to be demanded.

Judging moral climate action correctly

It is sometimes assumed and even claimed that the extravagant lifestyle we live proves that it is impossible to solve the climate crises. The argument is familiar. It may sound like this:

«People are simply not ready to give up flying, driving or eating meat.»

This assertion is suggested by climate preaching. It is supported by people’s unwillingness to respond positively to climate preaching. Because people don’t respond positively to preaching, it is claimed that they would not want any change to address the climate issue. This assertion is welcome to many, particularly many politicians. But it is plain wrong. A correct statement would be:

«People are, for very good reason, not ready to give up flying, driving or eating meat if their doing so does not solve any problem at all, but only disadvantages them selectively

There is a huge difference between the correct longer and the shorter false statements.

We should not judge anybody over whether he or she lives a green or a polluting lifestyle. Rather, we should judge people over whether they are ready to compromise on comfort and lifestyle choices if it solves a problem —only then.

Moreover, or to be more precise, we should judge people’s individual behavior over whether they support policy that will get enough people to invest differently, to live differently and to give up the whole of the energy production system based on fossil fuels and replace it with clean sources of energy.

We should judge people’s individual behavior exclusively over how actively they get personally involved to get policy enacted which achieves this goal.

We must stop asking people to be idiots. We must ask them not to be idiots, according to the old meaning of the word. (In democratic Athens the word idiot stood for a citizen who abstained from politics even though having the opportunity to be politically involved.)

Science and technology aversion

Science and technology have advanced humankind, no doubt about that, but not only in the right direction. Long ago, agriculture has started to bring about deforestation, soil degradation, natural habitat loss, extreme social disparity, diseases and industrial scale animal suffering. The industrial revolution has polluted water and air, nearly swept away the ozone layer and delivered climate change. The most amazing advancement of science has brought about nuclear meltdowns and overkill.

It is comprehensible that many are critical of technological solutions or even critical of scientific approaches to cope with global warming. It is comprehensible that many want solutions to new problems to go in a different direction, away from ever more science, technology and stuff. A somewhat romantic desire to do with less science and technology might also resonate with both the „less is more“ paradigm in terms of consumption and our other romantic wish that people act altruistically, voluntarily, personally, socially responsibly —all of which resonates with a desire to do with less regulation and with climate preaching.

Is there a Stone Age solution for a modern problem?

It would be wrong to posit that there is no justification for appeals for personal voluntary action at all. This article should not be interpreted as a general objection to appeals. This article goes as far as to claim that appeals for voluntary personal behavior change are more detrimental than beneficial in the case of fossil energies and climate change, a global problem.

It would be wrong to posit that climate preaching has no positive direct effect at all. It certainly has positive direct impact in one segment of the population. But both the segment and its impact are small and indirect negative effects should also be accounted for.

Essentially, the shortcomings of climate preaching and our belief in climate preaching are due to the fact that we live in large anonymous civilizations in a global economy, not in small groups of territorial hunter-gatherers. With climate preaching, we try to address a problem of a modern global anonymous civilization with a method that would be well suited to solve problems of small, naturally living groups as they existed in the early Stone Age.

To address the climate crisis is not about satisfying our moral instincts to preach for moral behavior and appreciate the preaching. It is not about our innate desire to impress or please those around us.

To properly address the climate crisis is to prevent severe and irreversible problems for nearly all life on earth in the somewhat distant future and the far future.

I’ve been arguing that climate preaching won’t help us in this respect. But opinions diverge. The main caveat is described in the next section.

The scientific case for climate preaching

Environmental psychologists and marketing specialists have investigated the potential of environmental preaching on consumer action (essentially on consumer action alone!) and continue to seek to optimize results. And results keep being mixed. One problem with these studies is that they seek positive results, and occasionally find some, but systematically ignore potential negative effects.

Also, these studies often don’t measure the real effect of a change or intervention but investigate people’s altered opinion or intention.

Furthermore, the measurement of the results may impact the results themselves, because the measurement is often a kind of social control. Sometimes even the effect of social control is measured when social control is imposed as part of the experimental setup, but inexistent in real life. (My favorite example: If you tell people that their neighbors use less electricity than they do, people reduce their consumption. Think about how meaningless such an experiment is. The positive results are nevertheless taken seriously as a case for preaching, although the experiment tested social control.)

Yet another severe measurement problem is that long term effects —positive or negative— are practically impossible to assess.

Despite the difficulties to assess its net effect, there is a scientific case for climate preaching. There is a theory.

Self-perception theory posits that a person committing an environmental act sees itself as an environmentalist and will continue to act accordingly. However, unless the act is self-motivated or believed to be self-motivated it won’t contribute positively to self-perception. But climate preaching undermines the individual’s perception of his green acts as self-motivated.

It is easy to spot more possible flaws with the validity of a simplistic appreciation of self-perception theory for climate mitigation. All perception is relative and the theory works in both directions.

People are told that flying is bad and that they should not do it, but nearly everybody does it anyway. Are the people addressed by the appeals not to fly likely to identify as environmentalists? There is little reason to expect so, rather the opposite. Self-perception is a nice theory. Climate preaching may have a positive effect on one small segment, the already strongly convinced. But for the others, climate preaching generates conflicting social norms and denial or frustration.

Self-perception theory may rather be a subterfuge than a justification for preaching. In any case, after decades of streaming appeals to the public, it is time to acknowledge the appeals‘ failure to deliver —as well as the backfiring and outright backlash against environmentalism. It is time for a critical review of the theory or its abandonment for climate communication. The minimum to do now is to seriously consider the downsides of climate preaching.

The effect of climate preaching on political apathy

Adherents of self-perception theory might claim as an achievement that there is fundamentally a certain degree of public support for protecting the environment. (To what extent preaching contributed to this result remains an open question, though.) However, when political action is proposed in the form of bills or laws that restrict individual freedom to pollute, opposition against virtually any environmental legal proposal is quick to mount.

Belief in self-perception theory might have obstructed our readiness to envision downsides of climate preaching.

For instance, it is overlooked that climate preaching posits and promotes eco-liberalism, the attitude that everybody is free to do what he or she pleases in terms of the environment. It can reasonably be guessed that climate preaching promotes political apathy rather than political action on climate change.

Unless one wants to strengthen political climate apathy is doesn’t make sense to promote eco-liberalism.

Unless one wants to strengthen climate denial, it doesn’t make sense to (implicitly!) accuse average citizens of wrongdoing because they happen to also be consumers and direct emitters of CO2.

Citizens should be treated cautiously, because they are needed as political actors, in which role they could make a difference. They should be won over and secured as allies in the real, the political fight against climate change, not blamed and shamed in their role as consumers and driven in the camp of those who oppose all action on climate change.

But, with their climate preaching, NGOs and well intended activists keep accusing the least responsible, the least powerful, indirectly. Corporations, utilities and governments happily join the choir, some scientists too.

Schmutz im Heizkeller beeinträchtigt die Verbrennung. Deshalb zu Beginn der Heizsaison Heizraum säubern. Eine Anleitung für des Schweizer Bundesamt für Energie.
«Clean your heating room at the start of the heating season.» A government’s response to the climate crisis. The interpretation of the message as ‹your basement is filthy and you’re guilty› is unlikely to contribute to solving the climate crisis. | Advice by the Swiss Federal Office of Energy Energy (disturbingly similar to US advice to prevent damage in case of a nuclear strike).

Incessantly, green shopping is advocated and we are told to calculate our personal carbon footprint or to take shorter showers, etc. While some pieces of advice about investing in a heat pump or solar panels, rather than another oil boiler, or cutting air travel may at least be somewhat meaningful, some governments dare to give ridiculously meaningless recommendations (picture above). A call to have fewer children may be at the top of one list of appeals.

Unlike what most people think, not a strictly rational assessment of a situation triggers action. Emotions do. Like fear, anger is another strong motivator for action —as is demonstrated by the conspicuous zeal of many active deniers. But how could climate activists and ordinary citizens concerned about global warming develop anger and direct it correctly at their real opponents if they believe that climate change is their fault? They won’t develop a Feindbild, they won’t know their enemy, if they keep being told and keep accepting that climate change is their own fault. I find it hard to believe they will ever win the ‚fight‘ over climate change, if they keep fighting themselves. (It is not surprising that, often at best, climate activists see corporations as their foes. Occasionally this assessment is correct, but rarely because of corporate pollution. It is correct if corporations influence environmental politics, which they often do, or if they disrespect the law.)

The paralyzing effect of climate preaching

Following Greta Thunberg’s example, in December 2018, thousands of young Swiss students took to the streets instead of going to school. Soon, on social media, among few comments in the Instagram page of the strike, user «Marcrihs» wrote:

marcrihs I think this is basically a good thing. But I think before starting to demand from others one should start with oneself. How many of these 1.500 participants [in one city] likely have a smartphone, which they replace regularly? Produced in Asia and then transported causing pollution? Not to talk about the raw materials in these phones, and not to start talking about recycling. Who of them flies for vacation? Who doesn’t repeatedly use PET bottles? I am aware of my duty and shortcomings and that I am not necessarily leading by example. But then, I don’t take to the street and demand it from others.

No, this is not an isolated example. It happens over and over again. Often, already in early stages of the formation of groups that set out to act in a relevant way on climate change, the groups‘ intention is undermined by members who just and only seek to avoid feeling guilty. Often these groups are rapidly engaging in discussions and a majority ends up accepting the proposition that it is illegitimate to make political claims if they themselves are not clean of CO2-emissions. Consequently, these groups often end up virtually paralyzed and abstain from meaningful climate action.

What psychology is at work when people like user Marcrihs make such seemingly well-intended assaults on the group’s effectiveness and question the legitimacy of making political claims on climate change? Let us be honest, everybody (everybody, Marcrihs too) knows that personal voluntary acts do not make any difference worth mentioning. We should not assume that the Marcrihs of this world really mean it when they suggest they do (want to) make a difference —Marcrihs preemptively cautions about his effectiveness. What, then, is the psychology behind Marcrihs‘ position, what his hidden agenda?

Deep within, the many Marcrihs out there know perfectly well that they themselves want to just escape an unpleasing feeling of guilt. If those immediately around them take on real work or take risks to address the problem, it undermines the Marcrihs‘ self-centered approach. People like Marcrihs would still have to feel guilty.

Actually, they would have to feel particularly guilty for dodging and free riding. To prevent it, they prefer to undermine their group’s valuable and truly moral intentions. Climate preachers sometimes even actively discourage climate action. They can be utterly destructive. By claiming to be morally superior —but being precisely the opposite—, they avoid risks, work and opposition.

The non-political case for climate preaching

For NGOs and governments, too, it is tempting to avoid opposition and instead proclaim what everybody can easily agree with: That we all should care about our own emissions and act voluntarily. No doubt, this is not their only motive, but it may be the primary reason for the NGO’s and government’s affection for climate preaching.

Humans want others to be altruists and perceive themselves as altruists. Climate preaching builds on this desirability —and the desirability bias— and resonates with it.

Adherents of moral appeals to counter climate change may rightfully argue that politics is the implementation of morals into written and mandatory societal rules. In fact, politics is mostly the transcription of moral standards into policy.

However, moral standards are not policy. Rather, one is the opposite of the other. The shift from one domain (morals, climate action as a voluntary individual act) to the other (climate action as a policy driven, collective endeavor) is exactly what we should aim for. It could take place without anyone putting the blame on consumers and ordinary citizens.

The unwanted consequences of climate preaching

To summarize, to this point, the effects of putting the blame on individuals by calling for voluntary lifestyle changes and ecological consumer habits are:

  • Insignificant action by consumers (at best, if at all)
  • Compensation, psychological or mental rebound (normally)
  • Rejection or even outright denial (worse)
  • Potentially recriminations, counter-accusations (in addition)
  • Climate silence, reluctance to talk about climate change (also)
  • Climate apathy, reluctance and even active discouragement to act politically on climate change (worst)

If it is accepted that political action is required, it should be further discussed how climate preaching interacts with political action. Or, are there other reasons in support of climate preaching that justify its abundance and popularity? This is the topic of the final sections.


The overall effect of climate preaching is negative.
Hence, it should be omitted. 


Part 4 — Climate Preaching is Detrimental Overall

It has been argued in previous sections that climate preaching has severe negative repercussions: denial, silence and apathy above some others. Is the net effect of climate preaching negative? In this part we approach this question.

Mainly, there are two possible caveats to the claim that climate preaching should not be done because it’s net effect is negative:

  1. Voluntary individual climate action on the consumer level might directly solve climate change, or significantly help to solve it.
  2. Climate preaching may not only have negative effects on political climate action. It may also impact positively on political attitudes and political action.

Regarding the first caveat, it has been argued before (above) repeatedly that it cannot be hoped for consumers to solve the climate crisis with their voluntary individual acts. For the unconvinced, there is (in note 8) a list of problems —solved or unresolved— which can serve as a benchmark to judge the (limited) human capacity for problem solving with moral appeals and voluntary individual acts. 8

The second caveat should be considered in earnest. Due to self-perception theory, as outlined in a previous section, or for whatever other reason, climate preaching might have positive side effects of societal scale.

Notable climate scientist and communicator Katharine Hayhoe recommends (to propose) small individual acts to start off with, hoping that a commitment to these acts enhances the readiness to subsequently engage in relevant action.

Similarly, if not equally, it may be argued or believed that climate preaching positively affects people’s minds and that their political attitude will be steered towards climate-friendly politics, whether (or not) they are inclined towards reducing their personal carbon footprint.

It could be claimed that citizens could —as a consequence of the preaching— be inclined towards at least easy political action like electing politicians who care about climate change. The failure of decade-long state and NGO preaching hardly supports such a claim.

Even if Katharine Hayhoe’s (and probably the majority of climate communicators‘) suggestion is correct, a positive effect would have to compensate for the appeals‘ severe negative effects (climate silence, denial and more) to make appeals overall beneficial. 9

Climate preaching is overall detrimental. Don't appeal for voluntary consumer action. | 'Flowchart'
Are calls for voluntary individual consumer action beneficial or detrimental to solve the problem of CO2 from fossil energy? The question is important because these appeals often dominate climate communications. Because climate preaching cannot solve the problem, its side effects must be considered to tell whether it is beneficial or detrimental (or useless). | Summary flowchart. It is not difficult to spot its peculiarity as a flowchart —or its flaws. While it is not to be taken seriously, it is nevertheless meant to make the argument of this part 4 of this article clear.

There is another possible caveat. Climate deniers are not only negatively affected by climate preaching. Deniers display considerable affinity towards moral appeals (it would be surprising if not) but there is harsh rejection of any kind of climate policy. Thus, the suggestion to solve the climate crisis with policy measures —in place of moral appeals— might incite denial, even more than climate preaching does. If so, together with political ideology, climate deniers are probably spurred by fear to be controlled by others, by fear of loss of self-determination. However, even if true, it doesn’t justify climate preaching. Furthermore, it is not an option to refrain from clearly stating that climate policy must be imposed. But these considerations suggest that we should not talk to climate deniers, because we just don’t win by doing so. Rather than to talk to them, we should talk about climate deniers.

There are more ways to (not) talk to climate skeptics or, rather, more ways to talk about them. One of them is interesting in the context of this article.

Excursion: How to silence deniers online

Often, when deniers flood social media content, it is nearly impossible to argue with them and be convincing. The deniers hardly ever give in. Consequently, they end up having the last word, the last comment in any thread, which is not a desirable outcome for climate communicators.

To try and prevent this outcome it may be explained that the denial serves to avoid a feeling of guilt. But the results of this strategy are mixed at best —my experience. The deniers then may explain that they don’t feel particularly guilty. (Of course you don’t feel guilty. That is what your denial is meant to achieve! You should not feel guilty at all when you claim there is no problem at all. Or do you, nevertheless, acknowledge that there is a problem?) And there we go, the debate continues.

However, I repeatedly made the experience that deniers almost always discontinue the debate if they are told that their ultimate reason to deny climate change is to be free riders, that they want to let others solve a problem and that their stance is an immoral one.

For example, an article (in German) about the discovery of the atmospheric greenhouse effect triggered a debate on Facebook. Nearly half the comments (39 of 94) written over two days originated from 19 climate deniers. There was the usual aggression and contempt on both sides. Some commentators patiently tried to counter the deniers by explaining the science of climate change; others tried with sarcasm —all in vain, as usual.

However, a series of three posts in the main thread (not as a reply to one specific denier’s comment) pointed out that the ultimate goal of climate denial, including the psychology at work, was free riding and that deniers necessarily believe in a conspiracy but that they are mislead by the fossil fuel special interests, not the climate scientists. These simultaneous explanations stalled the debate effectively.

To point out the motive of free riding works particularly well if a denier is not addressed personally but if the denier’s ultimate motive is explained to third party in the thread, not to the denier directly. The positive results I received in several experiments may be coincidental. I don’t have any statistics. Presumably, the outcome is not accidental. It may be explained:

By denying the problem, the deniers seek excuse and psychological relief from acting immorally. By telling them that their position is nevertheless an immoral one, their stance becomes less tenable. The deniers also seek to avoid criticism or even retribution for their immoral stance, their denial and free riding. By talking not to them, but about them, they are signaled that their intention to prevent criticism should be exposed. Actually in any specific case, their intention is thwarted. In combination with the accusation of ultimately acting immorally, their denial becomes unproductive and they tend to give up.

This communications strategy —if it is a strategy at all— probably doesn’t convince the deniers. It might even rather have the opposite effect. (If you want to be convincing, there is advice.) But experience shows that it quiets them. In light of the deniers‘ influence in the public sphere and ultimately politics, that may be more important than most people think. Despite the importance of silencing the climate deniers, let’s go back to the main theme of this final part of the article.

Climate preaching as a deception with positive or negative consequences

Supposedly, many believe that climate preaching has a positive effect on political attitudes and ultimately on political action. I believe this view is severely skewed by desirability. It is reasonable to hypothesize that appeals towards voluntary action discourage, rather than encourage, political attitudes and action.

It has briefly been argued in a section (of part 3) that climate preaching is a deception. Let me come back to this claim and view it from a different angle.

If, for instance, moral appeals lead to a significant voluntary shift from plane to train travel, as is reported from Sweden (in German), those who don’t fly for ecological reasons will take more time and may spend more money for travel than their counterparts who keep flying. This means that the reckless have an advantage over those who care and act morally, voluntarily. Is this the change we want to see in the world? I don’t know.

What if those who voluntarily cut air travel finally realize that, notwithstanding their leading by example, too many others keep flying and that the problem remains unresolved? Will their frustration ultimately direct them towards demanding mandatory change to solve the problem (the change I want to see in the world) or will they more likely conclude that the problem is unsolvable, despair and be directed towards political indifference or apathy on climate change? Truly, this time: I don’t know. We don’t know.

Climate preaching’s negative impact on political action

The implicit claim of climate preaching is that people should solve and are able to solve the problem with their voluntary acts, not the political citizens and politicians by enforcing rules. The adherents of voluntary action believe they «can do it» —and feel empowered as consumers. I have often heard them defend their claim by saying that politicians won’t solve the problem of climate change. «Vote with your wallets!», they and NGOs say. «We can and should vote with our wallets», it parrots from consumers —and, strangely, from many politicians too. «Politicians don’t act, therefore we do», they posit and act voluntarily, individually.

In our age of liberalism, voluntary change is generally preferred to mandatory change. If the (ill-)belief that voluntary change might do away with climate change —or be an «improvement» as is often claimed—, policy change might be considered unnecessary or less necessary, in which case appeals for voluntary action hamper political progress.

The promotion of ecological liberalism —voluntariness as an environmental principle or an environmental attitude— is compatible with the ideology of the political right, economic liberalism (Guardian article by Martin Lukacs).

As we now know for sure, decades of climate preaching did not guide the political right towards accepting and solving the problem of climate change. Neither did it make any political solution of climate change more politically palatable in that camp. It would have been paradoxical to hope for it.

The appeal’s usual emphasis on voluntariness undermines the legitimacy of policy work. If somebody demands policy, it is deemed necessary that the same person voluntarily complies with the rule beforehand. E.g. people tend to think that, if I use electricity from a coal fired power plant, I am not eligible to campaign against coal power.

For another example (Philippe Thalmann, May 14, 2018; French), the line of thought is: It is not legitimate for politicians with mistresses to praise family values; In analogy, it is legitimate for me to call on banks to divest from fossil energy finance only if my own fossil energy use is reduced in concordance.

The implications of this line of thought are severe: Whoever emits CO2 is not legitimized to call for regulation to end fossil energy use. If this line of thought were adopted we are stuck because this habitual —but ill-fated— moral reasoning factually denies anyone any legitimacy to demand indispensable policy change.

If policies are introduced to limit demand for flying to tackle climate change, how important is it that political leaders make obvious efforts to limit their own flying? | Steve Westlake Fig.6.11
Almost everybody already thinks that it is important that politicians are compliant if they want policy. | Source Steve Westlake, London, 2017, his Fig. 6.11

Unfortunately, this line of thought already prevails. Most people think that only who acts morally correctly himself may legitimately ask for policy change that will get everybody to act (graph above). Climate preaching contributes to this widespread but counterproductive mentality in the domain of political ethics.



An ill-fated but habitual moral reasoning is congruent with appeals for voluntary behavior change: Whoever emits CO2 is not legitimized to call for policy change to end all CO2 emissions from fossil energy use, people think. Climate preaching undermines the legitimacy to demand indispensable policy change.


The private consumer is the least powerful actor on climate change. Appeals for individual change put the blame on the wrong people, on the least responsible. Consequently, calling for voluntary individual behavior change shifts guilt and responsibility away from the truly responsible, the politically powerful.

As a consequence of climate preaching, most citizens fail to understand that they are also politicians, at least to some extent, and that their role as political citizens is crucial.

As a further consequence, even many of the politicians who see climate change as an important issue don’t do anything about it. Rebecca Willis reports: «None of the interviewees felt much pressure from their electorate to act on climate change. As one said, “I’ve knocked on thousands of doors, and had thousands of conversations with voters, and I just don’t have conversations on climate change.”»

For NGOs, state administrations and other climate preachers the way to go is to give up the failed attempt with preaching and finally be honest: Tell donators, citizens and potential political campaigners and activists that they are powerless as consumers, but indispensable and potent as political actors.

The reluctance to go beyond self-centered action

Only one very well known climate scientist, James Hansen, deliberately acts politically. The results of his doing so are stunning. Despite his success, hardly any other climate scientist follows his example, even though there are efforts to change that (by Naomi Oreskes, one example).

But many climate scientists choose self-centered action. They may acknowledge to change nothing, but portray themselves and claim to «tell a new story by changing how we live» and to «contribute to opening social and political space for large-scale change» (Peter Kalmus). Notable climate scientist Kevin Anderson devotes a blog to giving up plane travel and criticizes his colleagues who refuse to do the same.

The reluctance to go all the way with preaching and social control

At least one academic, Richard Wilk, goes even further. He suggests to accuse and shame the least responsible explicitly, even actively and systematically. As exceptional as this proposal is, it is consistent with the strategy of relying on voluntary individual acts to address climate change —if that is a strategy. If the strategy, or simply the combined voluntary acts, were to have a significant and positive effect, preaching, social control, shaming and blaming would have to be strong and ubiquitous.

However, the shaming and blaming of the least responsible would also have to rely on voluntariness. But social control, shaming and blaming are not beneficial for the voluntary shamers and blamers. (There is a reason why there is the implicit accusation inhibition.) People are not likely to volunteer in significant numbers to blame and shame their friends and neighbors. It will therefore remain a marginal activity. If not, it might do more harm than good, indeed.

Conclusions

Recommendations

Climate communicators are on a similar ‚mission impossible‘ as was Ignaz Semmelweis because the climate message almost inevitably induces a feeling of guilt and as a consequence reactance and denial. However, most guilt induction is unnecessary and could be avoided or counteracted. There are important lessons to be learned for climate communication:

  • Explain that the average citizen and normal consumer is not guilty of causing climate change. (Try to do it as your first talking point and you will find out just how much easier the climate conversation becomes, particularly if you address the not yet convinced.)
    • Global warming doesn’t have to be explained as an embarrassing moral failure, as an outcome of continued ignorance and human wrongdoing, as a result of human mischievousness or greed or as a lack of human capacity. It can be explained as being due to a lack policy and the weakness of global organization.
    • It may be presented as a global tragedy of the commons (the atmosphere) and excessive freedom in the commons, not the result of a failure of „all of us“, i.e. everyone of us, to act morally correctly.
    • It may be portrayed as a societal and technological challenge.
    • It should be presented as a societal, collective problem not a personal one.
  • Never call for voluntary non-concerted consumer action, voluntary individual lifestyle changes, etc.
  • Rather, honestly say and confirm the view that the effects of individual consumer action and similar voluntary responses to climate change are limited and that appeals are problematic.
  • Never ask anyone to calculate his or her carbon footprint.
  • Explain that CO2 emissions need to go to zero —fast. This should make clear that individual action on the consumer level or voluntary green choices can’t get us there. (Perhaps, if climate scientists had told us long ago that CO2 emissions from fossil energy must be eliminated entirely, quickly —which they should have done—, climate preaching would not have become the infestation it is.)
  • Talk about global warming and say who is really responsible for the changing climate. (The contrarians, including the fossil fuel industry, the deniers and those you oppose political action on climate change and the free riders who remain politically inactive or complacent.)
  • Recommend political action only. (Call it societally relevant action, rather than political action, if you prefer or if your audience prefers. Political action can well take place detached from party politics — a notable example is here.) Call for action to ultimately change «laws not light bulbs», as Al Gore used to say, before going on to please his audience with climate preaching.
  • Point out individual responsibility as political citizens only. Explain that political action is effective and necessary.
  • Neither exaggerate the severity of the climate problem, nor the difficulties to solve it, because the exaggeration also unnecessarily induces a feeling of guilt.
  • Abstain from dire messaging, catastrophism, doomism and only-so-many years left assertions.
  • Be prudent with messaging that might induce fear.
  • Focus on pride to avoid harm, to save lives and species, not on guilt for causing disaster and a catastrophe in the making.

The above recommendations are certainly unusual. Which points in the above list should be stressed, and which should rather be left aside or reformulated, depends on the audience addressed. Particularly, it depends on whether you talk to an audience of convinced or the unconvinced. The guiding rule should be: Guilt induction should be avoided. The easiest avoidance and most straightforward advice is: Don’t preach. And ask your audience to abstain from climate preaching, too.

One recommendation, generally accepted by climate communicators, deserves to be restated: Focus on real solutions and mention how technically easy and inexpensive the solution of the climate crisis would be. (Already very low and falling cost of power from the winds and the sun; Very low and falling cost of carbon dioxide removal from the air by purely technological means.) It would certainly be beneficial to also point out how just, fair and equitable the political solution could be. Concentrate on solving the problem. Counteract despair.

For obvious reasons, to incite hope in order to spur politically relevant action is psychologically tricky. But there is another recommendation:

Potential for improvement or pride

The framing of global warming as a crisis, an opportunity for genuine societal improvement, not just a need to cope with difficulties, would certainly be beneficial. Strangely, I am aware of only one person, Peter Barnes, who probably clearly sees global warming as a crisis, i.e. as also an opportunity for positive societal change.

Ignaz Semmelweis could have presented his hand hygiene policy as an opportunity to save lives, rather than a means to avoid the endemic levels of killing. (I ignore whether he tried.) A positive framing and a positive prospect could have been beneficial as it would have been better at avoiding the suggestion of guilt.

While guilt induction can hardly be avoided entirely, there are certainly better or worse ways to communicate mankind’s climate challenge and motivate effective action.

Greta’s strategy —a moral issue, indeed

Unsurprisingly, the combination of proximity (it’s about me, soon) and severity impresses and motivates people to act. Therefore, climate communications specialists advise us to tell people that climate change affects us personally, severely, soon or already. «The key is to show them that urgency is needed because climate change is impacting us now. It is not only in the future», they say (Scott Mandia) or: «Show them how climate change is affecting those things [that are important for them], here and now.» (Susan Joy Hassol). You should act because you are affected now, not only others later, they propose.

It is good to point out that new and severe weather events and patterns (and more) are a consequence of our changing the climate —of course. But don’t people know themselves, better than anybody else, what impacts their own lives and whether the weather does it already? They do. Skewed communication may work with some, for some time, but I don’t think it will get us very far in the end. (And it reinforces the position of the deniers!)

Also, they (and I) say that different people are affected in different ways by different communications. True, but we can’t always pick our audience.

I posit that those who are ready to accept that climate change is human caused like to hear moral appeals but the usual appeals don’t motivate them to act effectively. Furthermore, I posit that climate preaching reinforces the climate deniers‘ wrong position.

Is there no honest way to communicate climate change that works for all, or at least does no harm in at least one important group, or more? Just don’t preach on CO2, I recommend. But, there is a problem with that recommendation, or two:

First, there is a potential in appeals that should probably not be missed out on. Second, policy does not happen by itself. Politicians must do it and politicians must be pressured to do it. The latter is voluntary work by individuals or groups of individuals —or voluntary donations by individuals to pay for the work of others. Unless there is sufficient intrinsic motivation (there isn’t), this work or money will only be provided as a result of appeals. The solution to the challenge lies in appealing to people by saying that, yes, climate change is a personal moral issue, but of a different kind than is usually asserted and is usually thought.

We should tell people honestly that there is a moral obligation, indeed. That obligation consists of joining in and acting effectively, i.e. with effect. We should say that climate change is a distant danger for most, but a severe threat for many —and a distant but severe and inevitable problem for great many, unless it is resolved in time. We should simply be honest.

We should admit and emphasize that to let climate change progress is primarily an injustice. It is an injustice to other species, an injustice to future generations, an injustice to those in the wrong place and with small finances. It is immoral to let others solve this problem that must be solved, the problem with CO2. For anybody, it is unfair not to act with a political aim. Free riding is unfair. But free riding is what nearly everybody does, from individuals to nation states.

For good reason, many are impressed and motivated by Greta Thunberg’s simple and focused speeches. Isn’t it rather precisely this, what she says, albeit in different words? And, doesn’t she demonstrate that leading by example can accomplish a lot, indeed, if it is the right example? She does.

Having moved from self-centered behavior change to societally relevant action —a rare case, as already observed— Greta Thunberg stresses the importance of political action over individual voluntary behavior. (However, when she stated that she renounces invitations to speak if this implied plane travel, many environmentalists hailed her for it, unduly for specifically that.)

It is immoral to dodge real action with targets, then verbiage and continuation of fee riding. It is immoral to dodge real action with self-centered (consumer) action or with denial. That should be our sermon. If it is true, as I claimed in a previous section, that this communication (at least) silences the deniers, there is an honest and effective one-fits-all climate communications strategy. Not everything looks dire in the field of climate denial.

Yet, not everything looks good. There seems to be always more climate preaching, not less. It would take a lot to correct the course. That’s bad enough. But there is more trouble ahead.

A dire prediction

There are several claims made in this article. They range from:

  • Guilt induction and guilt avoidance are important for the communication, perception and denial of climate change (part 2)

via:

  • Climate preaching is an implicit accusation, induces guilt and, therefore, causes denial, among other problems (part 3)

to:

  • Climate preaching is detrimental overall and should be abstained from (part 4).

Theories may be helpful to avoid problems or make progress. Or they may be used to make predictions. If it is true that guilt induction supports denial, there is a prediction to be made. If the prediction is correct, the theory is supported.

As climate change progresses, it could be anticipated that climate denial loses ground and disappears. However, so far, this is not what we see, rather the opposite: Climate denial gains strength. As long as global warming was a distant threat, a future risk, there was relatively little reason to feel guilty about it and little reason to deny the problem.

Think, for instance, about somebody driving carelessly. He may have been warned about the risks. He may feel guilty but only a little bit, if at all. If, however, the careless driver hurts someone with his driving, he has much more reason to feel guilty. As long as a prediction is about a risk, a possible problem in the future, there is not much reason to experience guilt or reject it by denying the risk. That changes when dire predictions turn into reality.

It should have become strange to refer to high-intensity tropical storms, hurricanes and typhoons, as «natural disasters». Scientists are getting better at attributing the human influence on tropical cyclones generally, statistically, and even for individual storms.

Tropical storms now cause massive damage as high intensity cyclones are becoming stronger, as scientists predicted. But even in late 2018, the media’s reporting about these storms almost totally avoids to connect the damage to climate change. This silence is particularly noteworthy in the US. One reason for these omissions may precisely be that the storms can now be attributed to human influence and that they now cause extraordinary damage. (This silence is congruent with the arguments of part 2.)

As damage from storms increases, there will be more reason to avoid guilt by denying climate change. We are far from the end of climate denial —I dare to predict.

If my prediction had been made ten years ago, it would now be confirmed. The confirmation would even be stronger if the prediction had been made 40 years ago.

Nathaniel Rich reported about that time, long gone, when climate change could and should have been solved, when it went without saying that only a binding international treaty could do it and a treaty seemed to be within reach:

A report prepared at the request of the White House by the National Academy of Sciences advised that “the carbon-dioxide issue should appear on the international agenda in a context that will maximize cooperation and consensus-building and minimize political manipulation, controversy and division.” If the world had adopted the proposal widely endorsed at the end of the ’80s — a freezing of carbon emissions, with a reduction of 20 percent by 2005 — warming could have been held to less than 1.5 degrees.

A broad international consensus had settled on a solution: a global treaty to curb carbon emissions. The idea began to coalesce as early as February 1979, at the first World Climate Conference in Geneva, when scientists from 50 nations agreed unanimously that it was “urgently necessary” to act. Four months later, at the Group of 7 meeting in Tokyo, the leaders of the world’s seven wealthiest nations signed a statement resolving to reduce carbon emissions. Ten years later, the first major diplomatic meeting to approve the framework for a binding treaty was called in the Netherlands. Delegates from more than 60 nations attended, with the goal of establishing a global summit meeting to be held about a year later. Among scientists and world leaders, the sentiment was unanimous: Action had to be taken, and the United States would need to lead. It didn’t.

Then, Republicans and Democrats were not yet strongly divided over global warming. Exxon had not yet much reason to avoid guilt and the fossil fuel industry had not yet conducted its disinformation campaign. Governments around the world had not yet failed. Consumers had not yet been exposed to climate preaching. Nobody had yet been blamed— implicitly or explicitly— for causing the problem. Denial was almost inexistent.

Unless kids are told about global warming in schools at an early age, progress against climate denial will be slow or even negative. However, if they and we keep being told that climate change is our fault and that the solution is to recycle or to engage in similar voluntary individual acts, we cannot expect climate denial to be overcome anytime soon.

On one hand, scientists get better at attributing climate change to human influence. This development will keep making the denial of climate change less defensible. On the other hand, as climate change progresses and causes damage, the psychology that drives denial is nevertheless reinforced. The same development and essentially the same psychology spur climate preaching and personal greenwashing. It can be predicted that the polarization regarding the belief or disbelief in anthropogenic global warming will continue to worsen.


Notes
Note 1

The paragraph regarding «gentlemen’s hands are clean» is in full:

At page 631 of my work on obstetrics 2d edition, I have related the circumstances attending the practice of a physician of Philadelphia, who, in one of our epidemic seasons, lost a considerable number of women in childbed. I beg to refer you to that page, where you will see how he lost one of the number, whom he did not visit until she was advanced so far in the disorder, that upon the first inspection he pronounced her case hopeless. This case swelled his list equally with the first one he saw, to which it is not possible that he should have communicated the poison. His patients were scattered over a great superficies of the city and districts, some of them being more than two miles from others. At that time, many women were attacked, in various parts of Philadelphia, as well as in the State of Pennsylvania ; yet, so far as has come to my knowledge, no other medical gentleman happened to encounter such a great number of childbed fever cases as he did. I visited in consultation with him some of the very worst of the cases, and touched the patients, and was as liable to imbibe, or to be clothed with the effluvia from their bodies as he was; nevertheless, I did not carry any poison, or other cause of disease, to any patient of mine ; and if not I, then how should he become capable of doing so? He is a gentleman who is scrupulously careful of his personal appearance, of great experience as a practitioner, and well informed as to modern opinions on the contagion of childbed fever. Still, those of you who are contagionists will say that he carried the poison from house to house ; and if so, then you ought to give some rationale of the fact. Did he carry it on his hands? But a gentleman’s hands are clean. Did he carry a nebula or halo about him? Then why not I also? If the nebula adhered to his clothing, it might as well have adhered to mine. (Meigs 1854, par 173, p 103/104)

^ back to main text

Note 2

Meigs work documents the desire to reject the accusation that doctors, and in this paragraph Meigs himself, were guilty of spreading the disease:

I have practised midwifery for many long years ; I have attended some thousands of women in labor, and passed through repeated epidemics of childbed fever, both in town and in hospital. After all this experience, however, I do not, upon careful reflection and self-examination, find the least reason to suppose I have ever conveyed the disease from place to place, in any single instance. Yet for many years I carefully considered whether such transfer, by a third person, might be possible, and carefully read the statements of various authors to that effect. In the course of my professional life I have made many necroscopic researches of childbed fever, but did never suspend my ministry as accoucheur on that account. Still, I certainly was never the medium of its transmission. (Meigs 1854, par 169, p 102)

^ back to main text

Note 3

Reference to the use of chloride of lime as a measure against puerperal fever already in 1829 can be found in Meigs 1854. The respective paragraph is:

I with great satisfaction here refer you to Dr. Robert Collins’s work, before cited, wherein, at p. 387, he recounts the triumph he obtained over the cause of childbed fever by purifying the whole hospital. In February, 1829, it was scourged with the epidemic. He turned out all but the most destitute of the women, filled the wards, in rotation, with chlorine gas; closing the windows for forty-eight hours during the disinfecting process. He painted the floor and all the woodwork, with chloride of lime mixed with water, to the consistence of cream, and left it on for forty-eight hours more. After this, the woodwork was painted, and the walls and ceilings whitewashed. The blankets, &c. were scoured and then stoved at a temperature of 120° to 130°. From this time, February, 1829, until the close of his mastership, in November, 1833, Dr. Collins lost not a single patient with the disease in Dublin Hospital, a result highly honorable to the distinguished physician and philanthropist. Where was the contagion fled? Dr. Collins and his assistants, matrons, and ward-maids were not chlorinized ; were they ever, indeed, private pestilences? (Meigs 1854, par 164, p 99/100)

^ back to main text

Note 4

A remarkable study on the influence of group think on the perception of climate change (among other issues), was reported in Advances in Political Psychology (doi: 10.1111/pops.12244). Dan M. Kahan from Yale University: Climate-Science Communication and the Measurement Problem. (Link)

^ back to main text

Note 5

Many advocates of veganism or vegetarianism claim meat to be the most important contributor to global warming. This false claim was stirred up and brought to a wider audience by the movie «Cowspiracy». The claim is not even remotely plausible but refuses to go away —like many of the false claims made by climate deniers.

«Cowspiracy» relied heavily on a report published by Lester Brown’s Worldwatch Institute, authored by Robert Goodland and Jeff Anhang. Much in this report is so wrong that a number of scientists felt obliged to set the record straight. However, that «Cowspiracy» relied on other people’s obviously false (if not intentionally deceptive) work, is no excuse.

Another good exposition of important falsehoods presented in «Cowspiracy» is here. There is no need to recapitulate the debunking. Nevertheless, one big false claim is pointed out in the following paragraph because climate deniers often use the same false claim.

The authors of the Wordwatch Institute’s report count CO2 from livestock respiration as greenhouse gas emissions. However, respiration of CO2, whether done by humans, cows, plants, bacteria, etc., at most equals CO2 removal by photosynthesis. This biological cycle therefore doesn’t increase CO2 concentrations and these natural emissions must not be accounted for to point out the global warming effect of elevated CO2. The cycle is fundamental for the understanding of how life on earth works. Anybody who has ever been taught fundamentals in biology must have learned about it. Anybody who seriously wants to participate in the debate on climate change should long have understood this biological carbon cycle.

Some deniers also account for respired CO2. They do it to support their own false claim that CO2 emissions from fossil energy are comparatively minuscule —compared to greenhouse gas emissions from the production of meat. See picture below. (Fossil fuel CO2 emissions are the single most important contributor to global warming, not meat production.)

«The CO2-theory» with respiration. Shared over 1300 times.
«The CO2-theory. […] Fact is therefore: 0.038% CO2 in the air. [Deniers tend to copy the same outdated number over and over again.] Of that, nature itself produces ca. 96%. The rest, that’s 4%, humans. Thus 0.00152% of the air [is human made CO2]. And these 0.00152% should be responsible for changing the climate?» Stefan Rahmstorf calls this misleading argument an «ancient classic of the climate denier propaganda» and pointed to his post where he had refuted it before because, embarrassingly, even some journalists don’t get it, are not fit for their task, like to confuse the public or, rather, are victims of the implicit accusation inhibition. Like so much more in the denier’s information bubble, the «classic» just refuses to go away. Instead, the we-are-not-guilty message nearly goes viral. | Post on facebook by «Denkdeinding» on July 1, 2018. Within a few days it was shared over 1300 times and accumulated more than 400 comments. The Cowspiracists use essentially the same false claim.
There is another parallel between the makers of «Cowspiracy» and climate deniers: Both are inclined to believe in conspiracies. The Cowspiracists posit that NGOs that don’t join in and call for a meat-free diet must be part of a conspiracy. It is reminiscent of the deniers who posit that governments, the scientific community, many NGOs and much of the public conspire to promote global warming «theory», an alleged hoax. (When the denier’s inclination towards conspiracy theories was tested, by Stephan Lewandowsky and colleagues, and confirmed scientifically, the deniers were not amused. It would be interesting to see the results of scientists‘ probing into some environmentalists‘ inclinations towards conspiracy theories.)

Furthermore, like climate deniers, those who promote a meatless diet with the exaggeration of its effect are remarkably resistant to facts.

Vegans and vegetarians keep advertising «Cowspiracy», often after having been made aware of the falsehood presented by the movie. (As is demonstrated by this example of sustained promotion in spite of an acknowledgement to know better.)

In a «response to criticism» the makers of «Cowspiracy» somewhat acknowledge, but then nevertheless disregard the problem with their accounting for livestock respiration. The «Cowspiracy» website even keeps advertising the Worldwatch Institute’s report. The website does it below a bold title which reads: «Livestock and their byproducts account for at least 32,000 million tons of carbon dioxide (CO2) per year, or 51% of all worldwide greenhouse gas emissions.» Just below that section, the makers of «Cowspiracy» write: «Regardless of whether animal agriculture is responsible for 14.5% of GHGs or 51%, it is still a primary driver of climate change.»

Who cares about numbers off by a factor of more than three, as long as the movie’s goal to «raise awareness» is achieved? Not the makers of «Cowspiracy». They are well aware of the biggest of their many false claims, but prefer to deal with it casually.

Those advocating a meat-free diet to mitigate climate change typically live in economically strong countries. If they don’t base their argument on blatantly wrong numbers, they like to mention that livestock emissions amount to 14-15% of total greenhouse gas emissions. (This number is largely accepted. It strongly depends on how deforestation is accounted for.) However, regarding their personal abstaining from meat they should consider this: The fraction is only typically around 6% to 8% for those who live in economically strong countries. That is not because the people in these countries eat small quantities of meat. It is because their CO2 emissions from coal, oil and natural gas are terribly high.

The extra CO2 caused by fossil fuels and cement production has a very long tail in the atmosphere because there is no natural mechanism that could remove it in due time, for which reason these carbon emissions accumulate in the air, biosphere and oceans, unlike methane or N2O.

Unless CO2 emissions from fossil energy are stopped entirely with political measures, voluntarily refraining from eating meat won’t prevent global warming from progressing beyond intolerable limits and will cause human societies and animals much suffering. No self-centered act will halt the impending devastation of life in the oceans —we are not talking about individual lives, it is about many species to be lost forever and extremely biodiverse ecosystems.

Therefore, dear vegetarians and vegans with a tendency to overstate the importance of personal acts and a tendency to distort the facts: If you really worry about climate change, get political on coal, oil and gas! Start doing the right thing, or your conscience can’t be clean.

The above should not be misunderstood. First, and independently from all environmental considerations, it is fine to voluntarily not consume meat. Second, the production of meat is a huge environmental problem.

The production of meat usurps habitat about the size of Africa from the natural living world. This ongoing usurpation often destroys biodiversity. If not paramount, this is certainly an extremely pressing problem.

But let us be correct with meat and climate. And let us be reasonable with the appreciation of the impact of voluntary individual consumer decisions.

The «Cowspiracy» filmmakers‘ motivation supports the central argument of this article: Implicit accusations and our desire not to feel guilty are a dominating factor in how we talk about global warming (or remain silent about it) and how we deal with climate change (and fail to deal effectively with it). The Cowspiracy team’s motivation is reported in another «Cowspiracy»-debunking article:

The narrator/protagonist Kip Anderson’s sudden „realization“ that he couldn’t affect climate change after doing all the things he was supposed to–like turning off the lights when not in the room, watching water consumption, walking or riding a bike instead of driving, etc.–because animal agriculture was causing all the problem was amusing. He was just one person out of 7 billion people trying to make a difference, he himself couldn’t affect climate change just by doing all those „greener living“ things, though he would be a small part of the solution. Even more head-shaking was his „eureka“ moment with just one email from a friend about meat-eating, although legit, but the way that he and his producers went about to find out about it all was just simply wrong.

The author of the debunking article, Karin Lindquist, has looked into who made the movie and found out that all of its makers were vegans.

Should we just be complacent about vegans and vegetarians misleading themselves and the public with alternative facts on climate change? No, we should not. First, falsehoods are wrong —essentially by definition. Second, there can only be so many biggest causes of global warming. By elevating meat consumption to the doubtful status of prime contributor to the warming —or just overstate the importance of meat consumption (or meat production)—, the wrong cause is given priority and self-centered action on a smaller climate issue is prioritized over effective action on the paramount climate issue: CO2 from fossil fuels.

^ back to main text

Note 6

Guilt describes a feeling that is not only associated with unease but induces a desire to redeem, a desire to respond to a wrongdoing to try to restore the undesired state of mind. The feeling of shame is different from guilt insofar as redemption of wrongdoing is impossible or not considered. If that distinction is accepted, the doctors who opposed Semmelweis should probably have been affected by a feeling of shame, rather than guilt, because the lost lives could not be brought back. The distinction between guilt and shame makes sense when analyzing the reactions to appeals for voluntary climate action. Those who reject the appeals with denial might also do so because they consider voluntary individual action as useless because it fails to have any noticeable effect and they therefore deem redemption out of reach. This would suggest induced shame, rather than induced guilt. However, in public perception, the word shame as a feeling probably suggests a deeper, more conscious acknowledgment of the wrongdoing by the wrongdoer than the word guilt. Shame would suggest a more conscious reaction, whereas feeling guilty can happen with limited awareness of the state of mind. In this case guilt is more fitting in the context of this article than shame. Anyway, it is difficult to say whether appeals rather induce guilt or a feeling of shame and the article uses guilt where is could use shame —and probably rather often should use shame.

^ back to main text

Note 7

It would be surprising if there were no instinct that favors appeals. In our Pleistocene pre-civilization past all the methods people had to organize their groups were innate ethics, informal rules derived from these ethics and group interest in rule compliance as well as social control to enforce the informal rules. It could be called the ‚Pleistocene social toolbox‘. Appeals can be seen as the first step towards the establishment of informal rules. This is not to say that the ‚Pleistocene social toolbox‘ is absent or useless in modern societies and that appeals are generally invalid in the age of civilizations. But to resolve climate change, the old ‚toolbox‘ won’t suffice.

^ back to main text

Note 8

The limitations for humans to solve problems with moral appeals and voluntary acts can be illustrated with examples of different types of problems. There are examples of moderate success as well as cases of outright failure with appeals:

  • Meatless diet: Despite heavy preaching and campaigning, an innate inclination towards food taboos (a human universal), social control (to eat is a social activity) and animal suffering (against established ethics), there is insignificant progress or rebound on a per capita measure. The trend is negative in absolute numbers.
  • Sanitation: There is widespread failure to implement at least somewhat decent sanitation behavior in some areas of some developing countries. Difficulties to make progress persist despite strong efforts, the smell of human excrements and an instinct against open defecation close by.
  • Fur production or the wearing of fur: There are two very favorable conditions for preaching: social control and animal suffering. The problem remains difficult to solve on the consumer side. There is severe backlash after temporary success with appeals despite excellent conditions for moral consumer appeals.
  • Ozone depletion: There was also much —probably largely guilt induced— science denial including, in this case, among scientists. (It was a new theory.) Denial was also instigated by corporate misinformation. Attempts were made but failed to solve the problem by means of preaching. Appeals for voluntary action were ridiculed. (Quote: «Interior Secretary Donald Hodel urged President Reagan to reject an international ozone treaty in favor of asking people to wear hats and sunglasses. […] Hodel became a laughingstock, and consensus for decisive international action continued to build.») The solution was an international treaty addressing the production side. It was set in place including with financial incentives of unwilling nations through diplomacy. There were soon technical alternatives at overall negligible extra cost. However, as yet, the problem still remains to be only marginally solved, despite generally favorable framework conditions and American leadership —by the Reagan administration.
  • Whaling: The cause was never seriously debated as a consumer issue. Success came late but not one species of whales was lost. Still, the problem is not resolved for good. Fortunately —or not— a technological alternative (fossil fuels) came to help. The breakthrough progress was an international treaty to regulate production side, essentially a ban on whaling.
  • Poor hand hygiene in hospitals, still: Enforcement with measures that go beyond appeals are difficult to justify in this case and progress with appeals alone is slow. This is despite the Semmelweis story and indirect germ transmittance discovered and accepted long ago. Pride and social control, which is possible in studies, work much better than appeals and induced guilt.
  • Overfishing of the international seas: There is a strong emphasis on consumer responsibility but no solution in sight.

I believe that every of the listed examples are problems that are more easily solved with moral appeals and voluntary individual acts than the CO2 problem. This should make clear that CO2 is not the type of problem that could be solved with voluntary action on the consumer side.

^ back to main text

Note 9

Katharine Hayhoe’s suggestion that a commitment to a small act makes a commitment to a larger act more likely is certainly not wrong in itself. It is even supported by science: If you successfully ask somebody to put a small billboard up in his or her garden, the likelihood increases that the same person agrees to put a larger billboard up later. Moreover, the claim is plausible. It is consistent with the confirmation bias. However, will a commitment to a small, self-centered act also induce more consequential societally relevant commitments? My experience with activists shifting from political action to self-centered acts, but not in the other direction, lets me doubt it. One climate campaigner (Tine Langkamp) says that people who engage in the divestment movement tend to be prepared to go beyond this activity later and join actions of civil disobedience for example because, unlike people who engage in more self-centered activities, they learn that policy change is needed more than anything to prevent intolerable climate change. To propose small political acts as a first engagement, like signing petitions related to climate change or taking part in a demonstration, can be expected to be a good advice. Another recommendation is to join a politically oriented group like this (US, Germany).

^ back to main text


References, selected sources

(Meigs 1854) Charles D. Meigs. On the Nature, Signs, and Treatment of Childbed Fevers. Philadelphia. Blanchard and Lea, 1854. (Link)

Marko Rössler, NDR. Keiner sollte wissen wie er starb. NDR Info – ZeitZeichen – 13 Aug 2015 (Link)

Ignaz Semmelweis. Open letter, 1861. (German, Link)

Nina Kunz. Minimalismus. Lexikon der Gegenwart. Das Magazin, June 23, 2018, p 4-5

Paula Scheidt. Konsumkritik der reinen Vernunft. Richtig leben. Das Magazin, June 30, 2018, p 8-13

George Marshall. Don’t Even Think About It. Why Our Brains Are Wired To Ignore Climate Change (book)

(Feinberg and Willer, 2010) Matthew Feinberg and Robb Willer. Apocalypse Soon?: Dire Messages Reduce Belief in Global Warming by Contradicting Just-World Beliefs. Psychological Science. Vol 22, Issue 1, pp. 34 – 38. December 9, 2010. (Link, paywall; Link to full text, supplemental material)

(McDonald et al. 2014) Rachel I. McDonald, Kelly S. Fielding, Winnifred R. Louis. Conflicting social norms and community conservation compliance, Journal for Nature Conservation,Volume 22, Issue 3, 2014, Pages 212-216, ISSN 1617-1381, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jnc.2013.11.005. (Link, paywall)

(Stoll-Kleemann et al. 2001) Susanne Stoll-Kleemann, Tim O’Riordan, Carlo C. Jaeger. The psychology of denial concerning climate mitigation measures: evidence from Swiss focus groups. Global Environmental Change, Volume 11, Issue 2, 2001, Pages 107-117, ISSN 0959-3780, https://doi.org/10.1016/S0959-3780(00)00061-3. July 2001. (Link, paywall; download from research gate)

(Napier et al. 2018) Jaime L. Napier, Julie Huang, Andrew J. Vonasch, John A. Bargh. Superheroes for change: Physical safety promotes socially (but not economically) progressive attitudes among conservatives. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol., 48: 187–195. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2315.

(Stoknes 2014) Per Espen Stoknes. Rethinking climate communications and the “psychological climate paradox”. Energy Research & Social Science. 1. 161–170. 10.1016/j.erss.2014.03.007. 13 April, 2014 (download full text from research gate)

(Bissing-Olsen et al. 2016) Megan J. Bissing-Olson, Kelly S. Fielding, Aarti Iyer. Experiences of pride, not guilt, predict pro-environmental behavior when pro-environmental descriptive norms are more positive. 5 January, 2016. (Link, paywall)

(Schneider et al. 2017) Claudia R. Schneider, Lisa Zaval, Elke U. Weber, Ezra M. Markowitz. The influence of anticipated pride and guilt on proenvironmental decision making. PLOS. 30 November, 2017 (Link to full text)

(Obradovich and Guenther 2016) Nick Obradovich, Scott M. Guenther. Collective responsibility amplifies mitigation behaviors. 4 May, 2016. (Link, paywall). Online articles here and there.


Acknowledgements: I thank Kathrin Ernst, Victor Garcia, Jonas Hostettler, Karin Kirk, Winnifred Louis, Claudia Schneider, Maarten Willemsen and Bärbel Winkler for reviewing the draft and helping to significantly improve the article.

Keywords: Ecological lifestyle, green consumption, climate appeals, climate preaching, implicit accusation, induced guilt, climate denial, climate silence, personal greenwashing, political psychology, climate change, global warming.

Twitter tag: #StopClimatePreaching

Short hyperlink to this article: http://bit.ly/guilt-denial

Featured image: With its products and name the restaurant chain «not guilty» employs the inherent human desire to avoid feeling guilty.


This article keeps being updated and amended. Last update:
2019-01-18

Schreibe einen Kommentar

Deine E-Mail-Adresse wird nicht veröffentlicht. Erforderliche Felder sind mit * markiert